2013
DOI: 10.1111/bjso.12044
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Power and revenge

Abstract: In press, British Journal of Social Psychology Power and revenge 2 AbstractWe took an individual difference approach to explain revenge tendencies in powerholders.Across four experimental studies, chronically powerless individuals sought more revenge than chronically powerful individuals following a high power episode (Studies 1 and 2), when striking a powerful pose (Study 3), and when making a powerful hand gesture (Study 4). This relationship vanished when participants were not exposed to incidental power. A… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
33
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 41 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 68 publications
3
33
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Furthermore, work by Stamkou, van Kleef, Homan, and Galinsky () suggests that the motivation to achieve hierarchical differentiation can affect individuals' responses to transgressions in low power distance cultural settings (see also Mooijman, van Dijk, Ellemers, & van Dijk, ). This is consistent with studies conducted with British and Australian samples showing that powerholders not accustomed to having power are inclined to seek retaliation against perpetrators (Strelan, Weick, & Vasiljevic, ). Future studies are needed to elucidate mechanisms and boundary conditions that determine individuals' responses to low‐ and high‐ranking perpetrators in different cultural settings.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
“…Furthermore, work by Stamkou, van Kleef, Homan, and Galinsky () suggests that the motivation to achieve hierarchical differentiation can affect individuals' responses to transgressions in low power distance cultural settings (see also Mooijman, van Dijk, Ellemers, & van Dijk, ). This is consistent with studies conducted with British and Australian samples showing that powerholders not accustomed to having power are inclined to seek retaliation against perpetrators (Strelan, Weick, & Vasiljevic, ). Future studies are needed to elucidate mechanisms and boundary conditions that determine individuals' responses to low‐ and high‐ranking perpetrators in different cultural settings.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
“…Relative to other emotions, angry prosodies elicited strong amplitudes and a distinct pattern of cortical activity in low power, but not in high power individuals. Anger is a dominance‐signaling cue, and as such the finding that power reduces the processing of acoustic anger signals is particularly intriguing, albeit consistent with recent evidence that suggests high power can act as a buffer and reduce individuals' sensitivity to hostile behaviors (Strelan, Weick, & Vasiljevic, ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 61%
“…For example, people higher on the social ladder-who may be higher in both achievement and power values -view justice as requiring more punishment and less rehabilitation (Kraus & Keltner, 2013), and people motivated to enforce status boundaries with criminal offenders more strongly endorse retribution (Gerber & Jackson, 2013). People higher in power are more vengeful (Strelan, Weick, & Vasiljevic, 2014) and punitive (van Prooijen, Coffeng, & Vermeer, 2014), and people higher in power values more strongly support retribution (Okimoto, Wenzel, & Feather, 2012), and are less forgiving (McKee & Feather, 2008;Strelan, Feather, & McKee, 2011). In addition, leaders' power and status concerns contribute to their punitive tendencies (Mooijman, van Dijk, Ellemers, & Van Dijk, 2015).…”
Section: Self-enhancing Values and Justice Orientationmentioning
confidence: 99%