2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.pragma.2013.04.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Pragmatically motivated null subjects in English: A relevance theory perspective

Abstract: Despite English being traditionally classified as a non-null subject language, there are certain discourse contexts in which subject pronouns may be left non-overt. While previous analyses of this phenomenon have focused on syntactic explanations, each has acknowledged that pragmatics plays a vital role in understanding their production and interpretation. This paper takes up where the syntactic analyses leave off, and offers an analysis of the pragmatic motivation behind null subjects in non-null subject lang… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
9
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
1
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Contextual cues may be expected to indicate grammatical person (of the first and second person ellipses, as compared to third-person ellipsis), as well as the intended referent of the implicit subject (for discussions, see, e.g. Haegeman 2013;Janda 1985;Quirk et al 1985, 845ff;Scott 2013;Siewierska 2004; Torres Cacoullos and Travis 2014). The type of fragment under investigation, however, differs from these uses by the systematic self-referentiality of the zero-subject across contexts: it is interpreted as referring to the user unless otherwise specified, in spite of the third-person morphological marker.…”
Section: Linguistic Formmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Contextual cues may be expected to indicate grammatical person (of the first and second person ellipses, as compared to third-person ellipsis), as well as the intended referent of the implicit subject (for discussions, see, e.g. Haegeman 2013;Janda 1985;Quirk et al 1985, 845ff;Scott 2013;Siewierska 2004; Torres Cacoullos and Travis 2014). The type of fragment under investigation, however, differs from these uses by the systematic self-referentiality of the zero-subject across contexts: it is interpreted as referring to the user unless otherwise specified, in spite of the third-person morphological marker.…”
Section: Linguistic Formmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In particular, quite common zero references in English only appear in fixed syntactic contexts, such as coordinate, gerundial, and infinitival constructions; at least this applies to the kind of written English we explore (cf. Scott, 2013 ). Syntactically induced zeroes should not be treated as a discourse-based referential option on a par with third person pronouns and full NPs.…”
Section: Corpus-based Modelingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…0 Locate 0 in handbag. (Haegeman and Ihsane 1999:138) As Scott (2013) suggests, cases such as (46)-(47) should not be possible on Haegeman's approach. Additionally, as pointed out in Ihsane (1999, 2001), the attested data involving omission in embedded contexts are problematic for any theory assuming a link between omission and the left periphery of the clause.…”
Section: S O M E Previous Analyses Of Missing Objects In Recipesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…7 Scott (2013) suggests that subject drop in the diary register is conditioned by pragmatic considerations and processing constraints and offers an analysis of the pragmatics of the diary subject drop couched within Relevance Theory (cf. Sperber andWilson 1986/1995).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%