2006
DOI: 10.1038/nature04297
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Predator learning favours mimicry of a less-toxic model in poison frogs

Abstract: 1 . Mimetic advantage is considered to be dependent on frequency because an increase in mimic abundance leads to breakdown of the warning signal 2,3 . Where multiple toxic species are available, batesian polymorphism 4 is predicted-that is, mimics diversify to match sympatric models. Despite the prevalence of batesian mimicry in nature 5 , batesian polymorphism is relatively rare 6 . Here we explore a poison-frog mimicry complex comprising two parapatric models and a geographically dimorphic mimic that shows m… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

6
128
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 159 publications
(134 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
6
128
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Under these circumstances, a strongly positive, density-driven signal effect (density-dependent generalized avoidance, ref. 24; or another type of Allee effect refs. 25 and 26) may grant novel color morphs a higher likelihood of survival, enabling exploration of an adaptive landscape otherwise made inaccessible by purifying selection.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Under these circumstances, a strongly positive, density-driven signal effect (density-dependent generalized avoidance, ref. 24; or another type of Allee effect refs. 25 and 26) may grant novel color morphs a higher likelihood of survival, enabling exploration of an adaptive landscape otherwise made inaccessible by purifying selection.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Birds use bitterness to estimate the level of toxicity (Skelhorn & Rowe, 2010), and cardenolides are bitter in taste (Malcolm, 1994). The level of toxicity correlates with the taste and birds learn to reject highly distasteful prey faster than less distasteful prey (Holen, 2013), and moderately toxic species are favored over highly toxic species in learning (Darst & Cummings, 2006). However, once learned, birds avoid prey types that are likely above certain level of toxicity or bitterness (Lindström, Lyytinen, Mappes, & Ojala, 2006).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The negative effect on models is due to frequency-dependent selection: if mimics exist in large numbers, the predators may take longer to learn an aversion or the potential for evolving innate fear of dangerous prey is lessened. Although studies of the Batesian mimicry have usually emphasised learning as a mechanism for the evolution of mimicry (e.g., [3]), both innate and learned fear of dangerous or distasteful prey can favour the evolution of the Batesian mimicry, as is clear from studies using naïve jumping spiders (Salticidae) as potential predators (e.g., [4]). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet polymorphism can also be maintained in populations of the Batesian mimics [17], particularly when more than one model species is available [3]. It is especially noticeable that several species of ant mimics are polymorphic [18,19].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation