2012
DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1953-11.2012
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Predicting Conceptual Processing Capacity from Spontaneous Neuronal Activity of the Left Middle Temporal Gyrus

Abstract: Conceptual processing is a crucial brain function for humans. Past research using neuropsychological and task-based functional brainimaging paradigms indicates that widely distributed brain regions are related to conceptual processing. Here, we explore the potential contribution of intrinsic or spontaneous brain activity to conceptual processing by examining whether resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) signals can account for individual differences in the conceptual processing efficien… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

14
133
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 167 publications
(148 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
(64 reference statements)
14
133
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The extent of damage to these tracts correlated with the degree of impairment in using tools. The tracts that we identified overlap with the anterior/middle parts of left SLF, which has been shown to be involved in tool use (Ramayya et al, 2010;Hoeren et al, 2013) and various language functions such as syntactic processing (Wilson et al, 2011), phonological encoding in speech production (Schwartz et al, 2012;Han et al, 2014), and verbal working memory (Meyer et al, 2014). The present study further confirms that this tract causally contributes to tool use and, more critically, identifies the exact GM regions being connected by this pathway that contribute to tool use.…”
Section: Tool Use Structural Networkmentioning
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The extent of damage to these tracts correlated with the degree of impairment in using tools. The tracts that we identified overlap with the anterior/middle parts of left SLF, which has been shown to be involved in tool use (Ramayya et al, 2010;Hoeren et al, 2013) and various language functions such as syntactic processing (Wilson et al, 2011), phonological encoding in speech production (Schwartz et al, 2012;Han et al, 2014), and verbal working memory (Meyer et al, 2014). The present study further confirms that this tract causally contributes to tool use and, more critically, identifies the exact GM regions being connected by this pathway that contribute to tool use.…”
Section: Tool Use Structural Networkmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…Tool conceptual processing ability was assessed by jointly considering performance on three tasks: oral picture naming, picture-associative matching, and word-associative matching. These tasks involve the conceptual component of the tool network but vary in the modalities of input (verbal and nonverbal stimuli) and output (naming and button press) (see similar approach in Jefferies et al, 2008;Schwartz et al, 2012;Wei et al, 2012;Han et al, 2013a). We used the composite score across these three tasks as the tool conceptual score.…”
Section: Behavioral Data Collectionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To rule out this alternative interpretation, we trained the participants in this study to use objects to denote actions across modalities. The cross-modality associative learning clearly demonstrates that the prediction signals must derive from higher-order cortical regions beyond either visual or motor cortex, which may be involved in processing abstract concepts in the middle temporal or frontal cortex (Binder et al, 2009;Wei et al, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…The resting state scan lasted 6 min and 40 sec for the congenitally blind group and 8 min for the sighted group (data from Wei et al, 2012).…”
Section: Resting State Scanmentioning
confidence: 99%