2016
DOI: 10.1515/iral-2016-9993
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Predicting pauses in L1 and L2 speech: the effects of utterance boundaries and word frequency

Abstract: This paper compares the distribution of silent and filled pauses in first (L1) and second language (L2) speech. The occurrence of pauses of 52 L2 and 18 L1 Dutch speakers was evaluated with respect to utterance boundaries and word frequency. We found that L2 speakers paused more often than L1 speakers within utterances; but not between utterances. Similarly, only within utterances, L2 pauses were longer than L1 pauses. Regarding word frequency, both L1 and L2 speakers are more likely to pause before lower freq… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

8
98
4
4

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 102 publications
(114 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
8
98
4
4
Order By: Relevance
“…These typically fully crossed the factor Fluency with the particular target manipulation at hand (e.g., disfluency was equally likely to occur before known vs. unknown objects; Arnold et al, 2007), which also deviates considerably from the natural disfluency distribution in native speech (i.e., biased towards more complex referents). Finally, although disfluencies occurring before high-frequency referents are unlikely (compared to occurring before low-frequency referents), they are not rare (De Jong, 2016). Based on these arguments, we propose that the present findings should be taken as a case in point: Listeners are demonstrated to be capable of implicitly tracking different disfluency distributions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 54%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…These typically fully crossed the factor Fluency with the particular target manipulation at hand (e.g., disfluency was equally likely to occur before known vs. unknown objects; Arnold et al, 2007), which also deviates considerably from the natural disfluency distribution in native speech (i.e., biased towards more complex referents). Finally, although disfluencies occurring before high-frequency referents are unlikely (compared to occurring before low-frequency referents), they are not rare (De Jong, 2016). Based on these arguments, we propose that the present findings should be taken as a case in point: Listeners are demonstrated to be capable of implicitly tracking different disfluency distributions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 54%
“…Of course, the 'typical' and 'atypical' distributions used in the present study represent rather extreme examples of disfluency distributions; they do not reflect the much more balanced disfluency distribution observed in spontaneously produced (native and non-native) speech (De Jong, 2016). Nevertheless, we think that the present findings obtained with these arguably unnatural distributions are highly relevant and carry implications for our understanding of the processing of more natural fluent and disfluent speech, based on three arguments.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 59%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…De Leuuw 2007;Clark y Fox Tree 2002), su distribución en la cadena hablada (Shriberg 1994; O'Connell y Kowal 2005) y su frecuencia de aparición, no solo en la lengua materna sino también en la interlengua de los aprendices de L2 (Dewaele 1996;Cenoz 2000;Wu 2008;García Amaya 2015;de Jong 2016), o bien han analizado los fillers con vistas a la caracterización idiolectal de los hablantes para fines forenses (Künzel 1997;Brander 2014, Cicres 2014) o para precisar su posible impacto en el rendimiento de los sistemas de reconocimiento automático del habla (Shriberg 2001;Vasilescu, Rosset y Adda-Decker 2010).…”
Section: La 'Vocal De Relleno': Concepto Significados Y Denominacionesunclassified