1983
DOI: 10.1016/s0272-4944(83)80002-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Predicting preference for familiar, everyday objects: An experimental confrontation between two theories of aesthetic behaviour

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
81
1

Year Published

2005
2005
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 98 publications
(89 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
7
81
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Purcell (1986) suggested that preference might be given to those environments which possess an optimal level of discrepancy from one's own scheme, in order to generate a mild form of psycho-physical arousal without activating negative feelings such as boredom or anxiety. Conversely, Whitfield (1983) stated that preference might be oriented to environmental stimuli that mostly resemble the subjective scheme. Here too, as argued for other cognitivist theories, the representation of the relationship between man and environment appears to be "cold", unable to account for the emotional complexity of everyday experience.…”
Section: Place As a Stimulusmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Purcell (1986) suggested that preference might be given to those environments which possess an optimal level of discrepancy from one's own scheme, in order to generate a mild form of psycho-physical arousal without activating negative feelings such as boredom or anxiety. Conversely, Whitfield (1983) stated that preference might be oriented to environmental stimuli that mostly resemble the subjective scheme. Here too, as argued for other cognitivist theories, the representation of the relationship between man and environment appears to be "cold", unable to account for the emotional complexity of everyday experience.…”
Section: Place As a Stimulusmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This seems to be part of a broader -prototype preference‖ effect; people also prefer typical faces, animals, and objects (Halberstadt & Rhodes, 2000;Rhodes & Halberstadt, 2003). Several controversial experiments have compared typicality against complexity and novelty (e.g., Martindale et al, 1990;Martindale, Moore, & West, 1988;Whitfield, 1983) with the goal of determining which variables better explain aesthetic preference (see discussions by Boselie, 1991;Hekkert, Snelders, & van Wieringen, 2003;Konečni, 1996;North & Hargreaves, 2000).…”
Section: Prototypicality As a Theory Of Aesthetic Preferencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, design newness of a product increases as the product shares less visual attributes with other products in the same category (Whitfield, 1983). This is consistent with the notion that design newness is the inverse of typicality-the degree to which an object is considered to fit, resemble and be representative of a product category (Boster, 1988).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 80%
“…As exemplified by the beauty-in-averageness effect, humans always perceive an average and prototypical faces (i.e., a face morphed together with different faces) to be more attractive (Halberstadt, 2006). People also favor typical music (Smith & Melara, 1990), cubist paintings (Hekkert & Wieringen, 1990) and furniture (Whitfield, 1983) in comparison to atypical counterparts. Other research has replicated this effect on other stimuli such as typical exemplars of dogs, wristwatches, birds, and randomly dot patterns (Halberstadt & Rhodes, 2000;Winkielman, Halberstadt, Fazendeiro, & Catty, 2006).…”
Section: The Paradox Between Typicality and Incongruitymentioning
confidence: 99%