15Action-stopping is a canonical executive function thought to involve top-down control 16 over the motor system. Here we aimed to validate this stopping system using high temporal 17 resolution methods in humans. We show that, following the requirement to stop, there was an 18 increase of right frontal beta (~13 to 30 Hz) at ~120 ms, likely a proxy of right inferior frontal 19 gyrus; then, at 140 ms, there was a broad skeletomotor suppression, likely reflecting the impact 20 of the subthalamic nucleus on basal ganglia output; then, at ~160 ms, suppression was detected 21 in the muscle, and, finally, the behavioral time of stopping was ~220 ms. This temporal cascade 22 confirms a detailed model of action-stopping, and partitions it into subprocesses that are isolable 23 to different nodes and are more precise than the behavioral speed of stopping. Variation in these 24 subprocesses, including at the single-trial level, could better explain individual differences in 25 impulse control. 26 27 28The ability to control one's actions and thoughts is important for our daily lives; for 29 example: changing gait when there is an obstacle in the path 1 , resisting the temptation to eat 30 when on a diet 2 , overcoming the tendency to say something hurtful 3 . While many processes 31 contribute to such forms of control, one important process is response inhibition -the prefrontal 32 (top-down) stopping of initiated response tendencies 4 . In the laboratory, response inhibition is 33 often studied with the stop-signal task 5 . On each trial, the participant initiates a motor response, 34 and then, when a subsequent Stop signal occurs, tries to stop. From the behavioral data one can 35 estimate a latent variable; the speed of stopping known as Stop Signal Reaction Time (SSRT), 36 which is typically 200-250 ms in healthy adults 5 . SSRT has been useful in neuropsychiatry 37where it is often longer for patients vs. controls [6][7][8][9][10][11] . The task has also provided a rich test-bed, 38 3 across species, for mapping out a putative neural architecture of prefrontal-basal-ganglia-regions 39 for rapidly suppressing motor output areas 6,12,13 . Given this rich literature, this task is one of the 40 few paradigms included in the longitudinal Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development study 14 of 41 10,000 adolescents over 10 years. 42Against this background, a puzzle is that the relation between SSRT and 'real-world' 43 self-reported impulsivity is often weak 15-20 . One explanation is that SSRT may not accurately 44 index the brain's true stopping speed. Indeed, recent mathematical modelling of behavior during 45 the stop-signal task suggests that standard calculations of SSRT may overestimate the brain's 46 stopping speed by ~100 ms 15 [also see 21 ]. Further, in a recent study 22 , electromyographic (EMG) 47 recordings revealed an initial increase in EMG activity in response to the Go cue, followed by a 48 sudden decline at ~150 ms after the Stop signal. This decline in EMG could be because of the 49 Stop process 'kicking in'...