2018
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0198136
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prefronto-cortical dopamine D1 receptor sensitivity can critically influence working memory maintenance during delayed response tasks

Abstract: The dopamine (DA) hypothesis of cognitive deficits suggests that too low or too high extracellular DA concentration in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) can severely impair the working memory (WM) maintenance during delay period. Thus, there exists only an optimal range of DA where the sustained-firing activity, the neural correlate of WM maintenance, in the cortex possesses optimal firing frequency as well as robustness against noisy distractions. Empirical evidences demonstrate changes even in the D1 receptor (D1R… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 98 publications
(194 reference statements)
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Several microcircuit models of association cortex have already incorporated INEAR-like [73][74][75][76] , and also IOPP-like 24,56,57,59 , inhibition to account for the growing evidence for these structured inhibitory motifs [16][17][18][22][23][24] , thus succeeding early attempts that featured only unstructured 55,[77][78][79][80][81] inhibition. However, despite the long history of simulating D1R effects on dlPFC-mediated cognition 62,[82][83][84][85][86][87][88] , to our knowledge, no study has investigated how D1R modulation of structured inhibition affects higher cognitive functions. In rodents and macaques, D1Rs have been shown to increase inhibition through fast-spiking 41,42 , non-fast spiking 89 , or vasoactive intestinal peptideexpressing (VIP) inhibitory neurons 90 .…”
Section: Mainmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several microcircuit models of association cortex have already incorporated INEAR-like [73][74][75][76] , and also IOPP-like 24,56,57,59 , inhibition to account for the growing evidence for these structured inhibitory motifs [16][17][18][22][23][24] , thus succeeding early attempts that featured only unstructured 55,[77][78][79][80][81] inhibition. However, despite the long history of simulating D1R effects on dlPFC-mediated cognition 62,[82][83][84][85][86][87][88] , to our knowledge, no study has investigated how D1R modulation of structured inhibition affects higher cognitive functions. In rodents and macaques, D1Rs have been shown to increase inhibition through fast-spiking 41,42 , non-fast spiking 89 , or vasoactive intestinal peptideexpressing (VIP) inhibitory neurons 90 .…”
Section: Mainmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Stress-induced activation of the HPA axis increases dopamine neurotransmission in the PFC and activation of the downstream signalling cascades that negatively impact mood and cognition [e.g., 316 ; see also 317 320 ]. Indeed, it has been shown that emotional stimuli can influence working memory processing, manipulation, and maintenance that depend on the dorsolateral PFC, which is inversely coactive with the ventrolateral PFC [e.g., 321 ] and involve dopaminergic D 1 -receptors signalling.…”
Section: Effects Of Stress On Neuronal Mechanisms Shared With Cogniti...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The impaired neurocognitive functioning found in bullied children might also emerge from altered cortisol concentrations (e.g., Vaillancourt et al, 2008 , 2011 ; Ouellet-Morin et al, 2011b , 2013 , 2021 ; Östberg et al, 2018b ; Palamarchuk and Vaillancourt, 2021 ), that in turn, affect neurochemical mechanisms (i.e., neurotransmission) of memory, mood, and behavior. Specifically, stress can impact dopamine signaling in the PFC-striatum circuits (e.g., Gamo et al, 2015 ; Reneaux and Gupta, 2018 ), which plays an important part in neuromodulation (i.e., the capacity of a single neuron to regulate wide-ranging populations of other neurons), reward-motivated behavior, reinforcement learning (e.g., Ikemoto, 2007 ), and representational mentalizing (i.e., theory of mind) development (Lackner et al, 2010 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%