2012
DOI: 10.1093/chemse/bjs062
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prenatal and Early Sucking Influences on Dietary Preference in Newborn, Weaning, and Young Adult Cats

Abstract: Early experiences are of potential importance in shaping long-term behavior. This study examined the relative influence of prenatal and/or early postnatal experience of chemosensory stimuli on subsequent olfactory and dietary preferences of cats as newborns, at 9-10 weeks, and at 6 months. Cats were exposed to vanillin or 4-ethylguaiacol via their mother's diet either prenatally, postnatally, perinatally (prenatal and postnatal), or experienced no exposure to the stimuli (control). Newborns were given a two-ch… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
0
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Flavour cues from the maternal diet can reach the foetus through the amniotic fluid and/or the placental blood stream (Schaal et al, 1995). This early exposure to flavour cues generally results in a preference for these flavours later in life and consequently can positively affect the acceptance of foods with a similar flavour before and after weaning, as previously has been shown in many species, including humans, rats, dogs, sheep, cats and even in non-mammals such as chickens and frogs (Hepper, 1988(Hepper, , 1992Sneddon et al, 1998;Schaal et al, 2000;Mennella et al, 2001;Wells and Hepper, 2006;Simitzis et al, 2008;Hepper et al, 2012). The flavour preference can be strengthened even more when the flavour is also present in the maternal milk (Galef and Henderson, 1972;Galef and Sherry, 1973;Mennella, 1995), whereby the continuity of flavour exposure is a likely factor for success (Mennella, 2014), as exposure through milk alone increases flavour preference in some, but not all species .…”
Section: Mechanisms Of Perinatal Flavour Learningmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…Flavour cues from the maternal diet can reach the foetus through the amniotic fluid and/or the placental blood stream (Schaal et al, 1995). This early exposure to flavour cues generally results in a preference for these flavours later in life and consequently can positively affect the acceptance of foods with a similar flavour before and after weaning, as previously has been shown in many species, including humans, rats, dogs, sheep, cats and even in non-mammals such as chickens and frogs (Hepper, 1988(Hepper, , 1992Sneddon et al, 1998;Schaal et al, 2000;Mennella et al, 2001;Wells and Hepper, 2006;Simitzis et al, 2008;Hepper et al, 2012). The flavour preference can be strengthened even more when the flavour is also present in the maternal milk (Galef and Henderson, 1972;Galef and Sherry, 1973;Mennella, 1995), whereby the continuity of flavour exposure is a likely factor for success (Mennella, 2014), as exposure through milk alone increases flavour preference in some, but not all species .…”
Section: Mechanisms Of Perinatal Flavour Learningmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…The ability to retain nutrient flavors transmitted by the mother's diet (amniotic fluid, milk) has been interpreted as a natural mechanism for the safe transmission of predictive dietary signatures from mother to young [40], [41]. In fish, knowledge on the effect of early flavor experience on later food flavor acceptance is scarce.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When testing older babies (6 months), a preference for carrot‐flavored food was reported if mothers had consumed daily amounts of carrot juice during the last 3 weeks of pregnancy (Mennella, Jagnow, & Beauchamp, ). Similarly, preferences for chemical stimuli included in the maternal diet during gestation have been reported in other species of mammals, such as rabbits (Bilko, Altbacker, & Hudson, ), sheep (Schaal, Orgeur, & Arnould, ), cats (Becques, Larose, Gouat, & Serra, ; Hepper et al, ), and dogs (Wells & Hepper, ; for a review see Robinson & Méndez‐Gallardo, ). However, in only a few of those studies was exposure restricted to the prenatal period, and most of them included continuity of the sensory experience during birth by maintaining the administration of the stimulus until the onset of parturition, or even explicitly re‐exposing the neonates or the mothers to the tastant after birth, during lactation.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 62%