2013
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068440
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Preschool Children Fail Primate Prosocial Game Because of Attentional Task Demands

Abstract: Various nonhuman primate species have been tested with prosocial games (i.e. derivates from dictator games) in order to better understand the evolutionary origin of proactive prosociality in humans. Results of these efforts are mixed, and it is difficult to disentangle true species differences from methodological artifacts. We tested 2- to 5-year-old children with a costly and a cost-free version of a prosocial game that differ with regard to the payoff distribution and are widely used with nonhuman primates. … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
47
0
2

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(50 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
(90 reference statements)
1
47
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The comparison between donor performance in the receiver‐present/absent conditions was chosen as the key determinant of donor prosociality, as the no receiver control condition allows for a nonsocial bias toward a particular distribution of rewards to be taken into account. This approach is typical of prosociality studies that have included receiver‐absent conditions as their baseline control (e.g., Brownell et al., ; Burkart & Rueth, ; Claidière et al., ; Lakshminarayanan & Santos, ; Silk et al., ; Takimoto, Hika Kuroshima, & Fujita, ). However, as well as comparing the number of high‐value donations made to each receiver to that made in the respective control condition, we also ran a series of Wilcoxon analyses that directly compared the mean number of high‐value rewards allocated to each receiver (e.g., head teacher vs. janitor; class teacher vs. janitor) within each of the three games.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The comparison between donor performance in the receiver‐present/absent conditions was chosen as the key determinant of donor prosociality, as the no receiver control condition allows for a nonsocial bias toward a particular distribution of rewards to be taken into account. This approach is typical of prosociality studies that have included receiver‐absent conditions as their baseline control (e.g., Brownell et al., ; Burkart & Rueth, ; Claidière et al., ; Lakshminarayanan & Santos, ; Silk et al., ; Takimoto, Hika Kuroshima, & Fujita, ). However, as well as comparing the number of high‐value donations made to each receiver to that made in the respective control condition, we also ran a series of Wilcoxon analyses that directly compared the mean number of high‐value rewards allocated to each receiver (e.g., head teacher vs. janitor; class teacher vs. janitor) within each of the three games.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Differences in task and methodological design, such as task complexity and cost, can substantially alter whether and to what degree individuals respond prosocially (Burkart, Rueth, & Stanyon, 2013; House, Henrich, Brosnan, & Silk, 2012; House, Silk, Lambeth, & Schapiro, 2014). For instance, tasks that require greater attentional demand toward others can inhibit prosocial responses (Burkart et al, 2013). Moreover, different reward payouts can also influence prosocial responses consistent with what we found in this study.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the fixed-role version of the PCT, a child is always either an Actor or Recipient. Prosocial behavior in this task usually emerges by about 7-8 years of age (Fehr, Bernhard & Rockenbach, 2008;House, Silk, Henrich, Barrett, Scelza et al, 2013b;House, Henrich, Brosnan & Silk, 2012), although this behavior does appear as early as 2-5 years of age when the attentional demands of the task are reduced (Burkart & Rueth, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%