2014
DOI: 10.1037/a0035569
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Press CRTT to measure aggressive behavior: The unstandardized use of the competitive reaction time task in aggression research.

Abstract: The competitive reaction time task (CRTT) is the measure of aggressive behavior most commonly used in laboratory research. However, the test has been criticized for issues in standardization because there are many different test procedures and at least 13 variants to calculate a score for aggressive behavior. We compared the different published analyses of the CRTT using data from 3 different studies to scrutinize whether it would yield the same results. The comparisons revealed large differences in significan… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
135
1
3

Year Published

2015
2015
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
2
1

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 145 publications
(142 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
(115 reference statements)
3
135
1
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Such studies often match violent and non-violent game conditions poorly introducing significant confounds [10]. Many also use unstandardized aggression outcome measures, with a demonstrated potential for false positive results [11] or which have proven difficult to replicate [12,13]. Thus, room still remains for experimental studies of youth using better methodology.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Such studies often match violent and non-violent game conditions poorly introducing significant confounds [10]. Many also use unstandardized aggression outcome measures, with a demonstrated potential for false positive results [11] or which have proven difficult to replicate [12,13]. Thus, room still remains for experimental studies of youth using better methodology.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Among criminologists, research exploring media violence as a potential risk factor finds little cause for concern (Felson, 1996;Messner, 1986) and reviews of risk factors for violence tend to exclude media violence as a useful predictor (Farrington & Loeber, 2002;Hawkins et al, 1998) As Bushman and Cruz stress, there are studies suggesting that exposure to violent media affect a number of responses related to aggression, from questionnaire measures to laboratory behavioral measures. There are, of course, studies that fail to find such effects, and an increasing number of alternative explanations for effects that have been found (e.g., Adachi & Willoughby, 2012;Elson, Mohseni, Breuer, Scharkow, & Quandt, 2014). Even if we assume that research studies demonstrating the effects of media violence on aggression are consistent and valid (some of us do not), media violence can then be placed among several other cues that have been found to influence similar aggression measures, such as references to weapons (Anderson, Benjamin, & Bartholow, 1998), black competition uniforms (Frank & Gilovich, 1988), words and symbols reminiscent of America (Ferguson & Hassin, 2007), and low blood glucose levels (Bushman, DeWall, Pond, & Hanus, 2014).…”
Section: Manufacturing Consensusmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We are also concerned that theories in media effects are often slippery and shifting when it comes to direct effects vs. moderated mediation and what variables count as mediators as opposed to ultimate dependent variables. We worry there is a lot of analytic flexibility with existing studies (e.g., Elson et al 2014) and we suspect it is fairly easy to find at least some effect that is p < .05 (e.g., Simmons et al 2011). Collectively, these conditions make many claims about media effects difficult to falsify.…”
Section: Preregistrationmentioning
confidence: 96%