The Wiley‐Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development 2010
DOI: 10.1002/9781444325485.ch11
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Pretend Play and Cognitive Development

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
55
0
2

Year Published

2010
2010
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 70 publications
(57 citation statements)
references
References 136 publications
0
55
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…using a block as a truck) resulting in the increasing use of imagination and pretend play (Lillard, 2002). By preschool age, they are able to enjoy and participate in stories because of their ability to remember and follow the story line.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…using a block as a truck) resulting in the increasing use of imagination and pretend play (Lillard, 2002). By preschool age, they are able to enjoy and participate in stories because of their ability to remember and follow the story line.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nine-month-olds already manually explore pictures in books; they feel, rub, even grasp at the pictures as though they were real objects (DeLoache, Pierroutsakos, Uttal, Rosengren, & Gottlieb, 1998), and they engage in more manual exploration of highly iconic pictures (photographs) than less iconic pictures (drawings) (Pierroutsakos & DeLoache, 2003). The defining component of pretense is the performance of activities in an “as if” mode (Hickling, Wellman, & Gottfried, 1997; Lillard, 2002), and infants’ manual behavior toward pictures reflects a response to depicted objects as if they were real. Moreover, even children older than we studied possess “iconic realism” in evidence when 3-year-olds report that even a picture (a 2D representation less iconic than our 3D replicas) of an ice cream cone will be cold to the touch (Beilin & Pearlman, 1991) or that jiggling a picture of blocks would trigger the blocks falling (Flavell, Flavell, Green, & Korfmacher, 1990) or that objects in a photo would vary if real objects were changed and vice versa (Robinson, Nye, & Thomas, 1994; Zaitchik, 1990).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the delayed non-matching to sample task, in which the participant retrieves an object under a cover with a symbol which is different to a sample symbol which has previously been displayed, children do not succeed until roughly 21 months of age (Diamond, Towle, & Boyer, 1994). More generally, this period of the late second year is a time when many profound developmental milestones are reached, including the production of the first word combinations (Anisfeld, Rosenberg, Hoberman, & Gasparini, 1998), the establishment of pretend play (Lillard, 2002), and mirror self-recognition (Amsterdam, 1972;Nielsen & Dissanayake, 2004). Our experimental design was a simplified version of the standard outcome revaluation procedure, in that the participants only learnt one action in the training phase, and groups were compared in their absolute tendency to perform the action, rather than in a tendency to prefer one action over another.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%