2016
DOI: 10.1037/pmu0000161
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prevalence of emotions, mechanisms, and motives in music listening: A comparison of individualist and collectivist cultures.

Abstract: Several studies have explored emotional reactions to music from a psychological perspective. However, little is still known about whether such responses are invariant across cultures. The aim of the present study was thus to investigate the prevalence of (a) emotional reactions, (b) psychological mechanisms, and (c) motives during music listening in a cross-cultural sample. Six hundred sixty-eight participants from 6 countries completed a web survey featuring 22 items that measured musical emotions in general … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
62
1
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 57 publications
(70 citation statements)
references
References 95 publications
(136 reference statements)
6
62
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, research into emotion stereotyping in music would benefit from investigating a broader number of music genres beyond the ones generally found in Western culture (Juslin, Barradas, Ovsiannikow, Limmo, & Thompson, 2016;Susino, 2015). Nevertheless, the present research supports the idea that emotion stereotyping for some music genres occurs with minimal influence due to nonstereotyped lyrics.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Finally, research into emotion stereotyping in music would benefit from investigating a broader number of music genres beyond the ones generally found in Western culture (Juslin, Barradas, Ovsiannikow, Limmo, & Thompson, 2016;Susino, 2015). Nevertheless, the present research supports the idea that emotion stereotyping for some music genres occurs with minimal influence due to nonstereotyped lyrics.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Surveys of the general prevalence of di↵erenct music-induced emotions place sadness among moderately frequently experienced emotions: In a survey of 141 Swedish listeners, sadness was ranked 8th out of 44 emotion terms in of the frequency of felt emotions in response to music [21]. Furthermore, in a cross-cultural survey involving 668 participants from 6 countries in 5 di↵erent continents [22], sadness received a mean rating of 4.04 (on a scale from 1=rarely, to 7=often) with regard to its frequency of occurrence in response to music listening, ranking 8th (out of 15 emotion labels) overall. In the same survey, sadness also emerged as the third most prevalent emotion (out of 15) in retrospective evaluations of the participants' most recent music-induced emotional episodes [22].…”
Section: Can Music Induce Genuine Sadness?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, in a cross-cultural survey involving 668 participants from 6 countries in 5 di↵erent continents [22], sadness received a mean rating of 4.04 (on a scale from 1=rarely, to 7=often) with regard to its frequency of occurrence in response to music listening, ranking 8th (out of 15 emotion labels) overall. In the same survey, sadness also emerged as the third most prevalent emotion (out of 15) in retrospective evaluations of the participants' most recent music-induced emotional episodes [22]. This suggests that while sadness might not be the most frequent emotion induced by music, it is nevertheless a highly relevant emotion label that is consistently nominated across multiple studies and cultures.…”
Section: Can Music Induce Genuine Sadness?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(ⅴ) 视觉想象机制是指音乐刺激构建的音乐形象激 活了听众的视觉意象或者是视觉图像, 在此基础上 诱发了听众的情绪体验, 情绪反应是音乐和图像之 间紧密作用的结果 [36] . 视觉想象机制涉及枕叶皮层 [9,45] [37,52] . 例如: 听众聆听习惯和聆听风格会使之具有 不同的注意焦点取向 [53] , 当倾向于关注音乐细节内 [39] .…”
Section: 其次 为什么缺乏音乐经验的听众 能够较好识unclassified