2015
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-015-0550-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Priorities for representation: Task settings and object interaction both influence object memory

Abstract: Following an active task, the memory representations for used and unused objects are different. However, it is not clear whether these differences arise due to prioritizing objects that are task-relevant, objects that are physically interacted with, or a combination of the two factors. The present study allowed us to tease apart the relative importance of task-relevance and physical manipulation on object memory. A paradigm was designed in which objects were either necessary to complete a task (target), moved … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Object recognition speed (Harman et al, 1999;James et al, 2002) and location memory Trewartha et al, 2015) increase after object manipulation. However, there is evidence that we are not necessarily better in recalling/recognizing active compared to passive objects (Brooks et al, 1999;Harman et al, 1999;James et al, 2002) and are not always left with a better spatial representation of our surroundings after active object manipulation (Kirtley & Tatler, 2015). Within a naturalistic, real-world paradigm our study investigated the role of active object handling on identity and location memory for objects.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Object recognition speed (Harman et al, 1999;James et al, 2002) and location memory Trewartha et al, 2015) increase after object manipulation. However, there is evidence that we are not necessarily better in recalling/recognizing active compared to passive objects (Brooks et al, 1999;Harman et al, 1999;James et al, 2002) and are not always left with a better spatial representation of our surroundings after active object manipulation (Kirtley & Tatler, 2015). Within a naturalistic, real-world paradigm our study investigated the role of active object handling on identity and location memory for objects.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Within a naturalistic, real-world paradigm our study investigated the role of active object handling on identity and location memory for objects. To the best of our knowledge, we are the first to compare memory performance between handled and non-handled task-relevant objects within a real-world environment, as in previous studies the nonmanipulated objects were either always irrelevant to the task (Kirtley & Tatler, 2015) or presented on a computer screen . We show that: (1) identity memory was not influenced by object handling itself, but relevant object were recalled better than irrelevant ones; (2) identity memory was highly predictive of location memory, as recalled objects were subsequently found faster (this was, however, only true for passive objects, as location memory for actively handled objects was not predicted by identity memory performance); and (3) relevant objects were found faster than irrelevant ones when actively handled, but this was not true for passive objects.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Shifts of task-irrelevant objects hardly affected reaching behavior. Task relevance has also been shown to modulate memory performance in natural search tasks in addition to actual object interaction 1921 . Moreover, breaking down the spatial coherence of a scene by shifting objects in opposite directions strongly reduced the effect of object shift 15 .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In comparison to memory investigations in which memorization is the explicit task, during ecological behavior it is not necessary to constantly instruct ourselves to remember everything in our surroundings. In fact, an ever-growing body of literature provides strong evidence that very reliable representations are formed after incidental encoding during search (Castelhano & Henderson, 2005;Draschkow et al, 2014;Draschkow & Võ, 2016;Hout & Goldinger, 2010, 2012Howard, Pharaon, Körner, Smith, & Gilchrist, 2011;Olejarczyk, Luke, & Henderson, 2014;Võ & Wolfe, 2012), change detection (Utochkin & Wolfe, 2018), visual discrimination (Draschkow, Reinecke, Cunningham, & Võ, 2018), or object manipulation (Draschkow & Võ, 2017;Kirtley & Tatler, 2015). Draschkow et al (2018) investigated the capacity and detail of incidental memory, instructing participants to detect visually distorted objects among a stream of intact objects (the incidental analogue to the explicit studies of Brady et al, 2008, andCunningham et al, 2015).…”
Section: Building and Using Behaviorally Optimal Long-term Representamentioning
confidence: 99%