“…Although conflict-adaptation and contingency-learning mechanisms are not necessarily mutually exclusive and could be integrated within a common theoretical framework (Abrahamse, Braem, Notebaert, & Verguts, 2016; Egner, 2014), in recent years there has been a debate about whether contingency learning alone may be a sufficient explanation for proportion-congruent effects, that is, whether these effects can be explained by an account that does not require invoking a mechanism of adaptation to conflict frequency at all (e.g., Atalay & Misirlisoy, 2012, 2014; Bugg, 2014; Bugg et al, 2011; Bugg & Hutchison, 2013; Hazeltine & Mordkoff, 2014; Hutchison, 2011; Schmidt, 2013a, 2013b, 2013c; Schmidt & Besner, 2008; Schmidt et al, 2015). More recently, however, some evidence has emerged suggesting that list-wide proportion-congruent effects do persist when controlling for both contingency learning (Bugg, 2014; Bugg & Chanani, 2011; Gonthier et al, 2016; Hutchison, 2011; Spinelli & Lupker, 2020b; Spinelli, Perry, & Lupker, 2019) and learning of list-wide temporal expectancies, another nonconflict learning mechanism thought to contribute to generating list-wide proportion-congruent effects (Cohen-Shikora, Suh, & Bugg, 2019; Spinelli et al, 2019). These results support the claim that humans do have access to a proactive mechanism of adaptation to list-wide frequency of conflict (for counterarguments, see Schmidt, 2013c, 2014, 2017).…”