PsycTESTS Dataset 2016
DOI: 10.1037/t51677-000
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Probabilistic Classification Task

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
5
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The magnitude of their learned spatial bias was comparable to adults' learning, suggesting the presence of a highly functioning implicit spatial attention mechanism early in development. This is consistent with findings showing early mature implicit learning (Amso & Davidow, 2012;Aslin, 2017;Finn et al, 2016;Thomas et al, 2004).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The magnitude of their learned spatial bias was comparable to adults' learning, suggesting the presence of a highly functioning implicit spatial attention mechanism early in development. This is consistent with findings showing early mature implicit learning (Amso & Davidow, 2012;Aslin, 2017;Finn et al, 2016;Thomas et al, 2004).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Generally, implicit learning has been hypothesized to have five distinguishing features from explicit learning (Reber, 1989): (1) developmental invariance (Drag & Bieliauskas, 2010;Finn et al, 2016), (2) robustness to disease or injury (Reber, 2013;Reber, Martinex, & Weintraub, 2003), (3) specificity of transfer (Manza & Reber, 1997), (4) IQ independence (Atwell, Conners, & Merrill, 2003;Bussy, Charrin, Brun, Curie, & des Portes, 2011), and (5) secondary task independence (Curran & Keele, 1993;Hayes & Broadbent, 1988). That is, implicitly acquired knowledge tends to be inflexible, perceptually bound to the training context, and remains intact with regard to aging, developmental maturation, neurological/psychological disorder, and lack of attentional resources.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The caudate nucleus matures early in development and is thought to be fully adult-like by 7 years-old (Casey et al, 2004). More generally, procedural memory and learning systems are thought to be fully adult-like by about 10 years old, in contrast to the protracted developmental course of declarative memory systems involved in the explicit system, such as the PFC and medial temporal lobe (Diamond, 2002;Finn et al, 2016).…”
Section: Information-integration Learning In Children and Adultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the declarative-procedural memory framework, irregular forms are thought to be stored in declarative memory while the regular form is thought to be composed in real-time by the procedural memory system (Ullman et al 1997;Ullman 2001). Interestingly, a large body of evidence suggests that the procedural memory system is functioning at its peak during childhood, then progressively declines from adolescence through early adulthood (Fredriksson et al 2000;Schlaug 2001;Walton et al 1992;Wolansky et al 1999;Janacsek et al 2012), while the declarative memory system shows the opposite developmental pattern (Campbell and Spear 1972;DiGiulio et al 1994;Kail and Hagen 1977;Meudell 1983;Ornstein 1978;Siegler 1978;Finn et al 2016). That is, children's memory systems are in a maturational state that favors the formation of productive rules over storing individual forms.…”
Section: Why Does the Tolerance Principle Work For Children And Not Adults?mentioning
confidence: 99%