1997
DOI: 10.1007/bf03395227
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Probability of stimulus equivalence as a function of training design

Abstract: The present study investigated differential effects of training design on probability of an equivalence outcome. Forty normal adults were assigned to four different groups. Subjects in the first three groups were exposed to a C-A equivalence test directly following linear series (AB and BC), many-to-one (AB and CB), or one-to-many (BA and BC) training, respectively, while the subjects in the fourth group, following linear series training, were exposed to a symmetry test before the C-A test. Three comparison st… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3

Citation Types

27
88
5
6

Year Published

2000
2000
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 105 publications
(126 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
27
88
5
6
Order By: Relevance
“…The current study has replicated earlier findings in which an increase in reaction time from training to test, and a decrease during the test has been shown (Arntzen & Holth, 1997;Holth & Arntzen, 1998a, 1998b. Fields and Verhave (1987) have postulated that as nodal distance increases, accuracy of responding decreases.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The current study has replicated earlier findings in which an increase in reaction time from training to test, and a decrease during the test has been shown (Arntzen & Holth, 1997;Holth & Arntzen, 1998a, 1998b. Fields and Verhave (1987) have postulated that as nodal distance increases, accuracy of responding decreases.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
“…Similar results with 3 three-member stimulus classes were found by Arntzen and Holth (1997) when comparing percentages of subjects responding in accord with equivalence following linear series, many-to-one, and one-to-many training structure. Following a linear series training structure, only 2-3 of 10 subjects responded in accord with a positive equivalence test.…”
supporting
confidence: 80%
“…In Experiment 3, equivalence outcome was tested successively following training according to a many-to-one training structure and one-to-many training structure with both 3 and 4 members in each class. In accord with the results of Arntzen and Holth (1997) the present results indicated a superiority of the oneto-many over alternative training structures as regards the . probability of an equivalence outcome.…”
supporting
confidence: 89%
“…In fact, several studies have examined various factors that may raise yields from stimulus equivalence training paradigms closer to 100%. For instance, some studies have compared the relative yield rates of one-to-many (A-B, A-C, A-D), many-to-one (B-A, C-A), and linear training (A-B-C-D) protocols (e.g., Arntzen & Holth, 1997;Hove, 2003;Smeets & Barnes-Holmes, 2005). Interestingly, however, the current study consciously employed quite effective procedures for training and testing purposes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…This is a method long understood to increase acquisition rates of conditional discriminations (see Doan & Cooper, 1971). In addition, a one-to-many training protocol was employed, rather than the less effective linear protocol (Arntzen & Holth, 1997).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%