2006
DOI: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2005.06.031
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Probing the limits of delay intolerance: Preliminary young adult data from the Delay Frustration Task (DeFT)

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
34
1

Year Published

2007
2007
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
0
34
1
Order By: Relevance
“…An examination of patterns of variance across cells in the analysis for patients less than 12 years of age suggests that ceiling effects were less of an issue in this age group. Future research with adolescents should employ tasks that either extend the delay element of LL (i.e., adjusting delay levels during the task in response to performance to maximize the differentiation between delay averse and nondelay averse; Mü ller, Sonuga-Barke, Brandeis, & Steinhausen, 2006) or adopt a completely different index of delay aversion (Bitsakou, Antrop, Wiersema, Sonuga-Barke, 2005). Interpreting differences between childhood and adolescents on the MIDA may be further complicated by the fact that the expression of DAv changes with age, as is the case with other symptoms of ADHD (Nutt et al, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An examination of patterns of variance across cells in the analysis for patients less than 12 years of age suggests that ceiling effects were less of an issue in this age group. Future research with adolescents should employ tasks that either extend the delay element of LL (i.e., adjusting delay levels during the task in response to performance to maximize the differentiation between delay averse and nondelay averse; Mü ller, Sonuga-Barke, Brandeis, & Steinhausen, 2006) or adopt a completely different index of delay aversion (Bitsakou, Antrop, Wiersema, Sonuga-Barke, 2005). Interpreting differences between childhood and adolescents on the MIDA may be further complicated by the fact that the expression of DAv changes with age, as is the case with other symptoms of ADHD (Nutt et al, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, children with ADHD are unusually vigilant to environmental delay-related cues suggesting an increased emotional salience for delay. They find the imposition of unexpected delay more Delay Aversion and ADHD -R2 8 frustrating than controls as indexed by an increased rate of responding during the delay period on the Delay Frustration Task (DeFT; Bitsakou et al, 2006). They show more activity and increased responding during fixed periods of delay or the extinction of reinforcers (Sagvolden et al, 1998).…”
Section: ) Of Two Tasks Commonly Used To Index This Tendency (Maumentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Delay Frustration Task (DeFT; Bitsakou et al, 2006): The task has been described fully elsewhere (Bitsakou et al, 2006) and has high test-retest reliability (intra-class correlation This pattern of delay following responses was designed to create delay-related frustration leading to attempts by DAv participants to escape delay by pressing the button to move on to the next question. The participants were 'warned' that the computer has given signs of malfunction and that if the computer appeared not to register their response they may need to respond again before they could move onto the next trial.…”
Section: Tasks and Measuresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the predictions of secondary adaptations are consistent with data from a range of different paradigms. For instance, ADHD children respond inappropriately to the unexpected imposition of delay (Bitsakou et al, 2006) and extinction of reward (Sagvolden et al, 1998); prematurely disengage from long and challenging tasks (Scime and Norvilitis, 2006); show more activity than controls during delay (Antrop et al, 2003); are biased towards task responses tied to immediate rewards (Tripp and Alsop, 2001); are unusually vigilant to environmental delay-related cues ; prefer reward immediacy to high reward rate or task ease (Neef et al, 2005); discount future hypothetical rewards (Barkley et al, 2001; but see Scheres et al, 2006 for counter case); and are differentially affected by slow event rates/sparse schedules of reinforcement Aase and Sagvolden, 2006). In summary there is evidence to support the notion that ADHD children can be DAv in both the narrow sense of choosing SS over LL rewards to escape delay in choice situations and in the broader sense as in seen in the effects of time-on-task/trial length on performance in non-choice situations.…”
Section: Psychopharmacologymentioning
confidence: 99%