2002
DOI: 10.1080/0144341022000023653
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Problem Solving in Geometry: Comparing the effects of non-goal specific instruction and conventional worked examples

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2004
2004
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
3
2
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Despite the benefits of participants' use of examples on the effective acquisition of problem-solving skills (e.g., Catrambone, 1994Catrambone, , 1996Chen, 1995;Ross & Kennedy, 1990) and the wide use of pictorial examples as problem-solving aids in a variety of educational settings (e.g., Doornekamp, 2001;Lim & Moore, 2002;Noh & Scharmann, 1997), there is a relative paucity of research on the specific circumstances under which the use of examples benefits or impairs the solver's performance. Most important, very few studies in psychology (e.g., Jansson & Smith, 1991;Smith et al, 1993) have examined the cognitive mechanisms underlying possible negative transfer effects following the use of examples.…”
Section: The Present Study: Overview and Designmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Despite the benefits of participants' use of examples on the effective acquisition of problem-solving skills (e.g., Catrambone, 1994Catrambone, , 1996Chen, 1995;Ross & Kennedy, 1990) and the wide use of pictorial examples as problem-solving aids in a variety of educational settings (e.g., Doornekamp, 2001;Lim & Moore, 2002;Noh & Scharmann, 1997), there is a relative paucity of research on the specific circumstances under which the use of examples benefits or impairs the solver's performance. Most important, very few studies in psychology (e.g., Jansson & Smith, 1991;Smith et al, 1993) have examined the cognitive mechanisms underlying possible negative transfer effects following the use of examples.…”
Section: The Present Study: Overview and Designmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although Jansson and Smith (1991) and Purcell et al (1993) provided evidence for the occurrence of fixation specifically in design students and professional engineers, fixation from pictorial examples might be a more general problem-solving phenomenon, one that affects both design experts and participants naive to design tasks. If fixation due to example designs occurs irrespective of the individual's area of expertise, it may reflect a broader cognitive phenomenon, the implications of which are critical for learning and instruction, given the wide use of pictorial examples in a variety of educational settings (e.g., Lim & Moore, 2002). A variation of Jansson and Smith's (1991) experiment was therefore undertaken to determine whether fixation occurs in a laboratory design problem-solving situation with participants naive to design tasks.…”
Section: The Present Study: Overview and Designmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Presenting examples that represent real-life problems helps set goals and supports development of metacognition that helps learners evaluate future applications of knowledge and skills (Lim and Moore, 2002).…”
Section: Guidelines To Support Self-assessmentmentioning
confidence: 99%