2009
DOI: 10.1075/eurosla.9.06fre
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Processing of grammatical gender in French as a first and second language: Evidence from ERPs

Abstract: The present study examined the processing of grammatical gender in second language (L2) French as a function of language background (Experiment 1) and as a function of overt phonetic properties of agreement (Experiment 2) by examining Event Related Potential (ERP) responses to gender discord in L2 French. In Experiment 1 we explored the role of the presence/absence of abstract grammatical gender in the L1 (gendered German, ungendered English): we compared German and English learners of French when processing p… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
33
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 74 publications
1
33
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, psycholinguistic studies of proficient L2 learners have demonstrated that grammatical properties of the learner's L1 can influence processing of the L2, including the computation of morphosyntactic information in real time. Violations of L2 morphosyntactic contrasts that are also expressed in the L1 elicit stronger neural and behavioral responses than those that superficially differ between the L1 and L2 (Frenck‐Mestre et al, 2009; Jiang, 2004, 2007; Sabourin & Haverkort, 2003; Sabourin & Stowe, 2008; Tokowicz & MacWhinney, 2005). A case in point is the comparison of L2 acquisition of gender concord: ERP responses to gender concord violations are found earlier for learners whose L1 has this grammatical feature than for learners whose L1 lacks it, although several factors come into play (Frenck‐Mestre et al, 2009; Sabourin & Stowe, 2008).…”
Section: Effects Of L1‐l2 Similaritymentioning
confidence: 98%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In addition, psycholinguistic studies of proficient L2 learners have demonstrated that grammatical properties of the learner's L1 can influence processing of the L2, including the computation of morphosyntactic information in real time. Violations of L2 morphosyntactic contrasts that are also expressed in the L1 elicit stronger neural and behavioral responses than those that superficially differ between the L1 and L2 (Frenck‐Mestre et al, 2009; Jiang, 2004, 2007; Sabourin & Haverkort, 2003; Sabourin & Stowe, 2008; Tokowicz & MacWhinney, 2005). A case in point is the comparison of L2 acquisition of gender concord: ERP responses to gender concord violations are found earlier for learners whose L1 has this grammatical feature than for learners whose L1 lacks it, although several factors come into play (Frenck‐Mestre et al, 2009; Sabourin & Stowe, 2008).…”
Section: Effects Of L1‐l2 Similaritymentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Lexical and semantic/conceptual manipulations such as semantic fit between a word and its preceding context (Kutas & Hillyard, 1980), lexical status (Bentin, 1987), semantic priming (Bentin, McCarthy, & Wood, 1985), word frequency (Barber, Vergara, & Carreiras, 2004), and probability of occurrence within a given word string (Kutas & Hillyard, 1984) elicit a negative deflection peaking around 400 ms after the presentation of a stimulus (the N400 effect). In contrast, a large positive deflection with an onset at about 500 ms and a duration of several hundred milliseconds (the P600 effect) is elicited by a disparate set of syntactic anomalies, including violations of phrase structure (Hagoort, Brown, & Groothusen, 1993; Neville, Nicol, Barss, Forster, & Garrett, 1991; Osterhout & Holcomb, 1992), subcategorization (Ainsworth‐Darnell, Shulman, & Boland, 1998; Osterhout & Holcomb, 1992; Osterhout, Holcomb, & Swinney, 1994) and violations in agreement of number, gender, and case (Frenck‐Mestre, Foucart, Carrasco, & Herschensohn, 2009; Frenck‐Mestre, Osterhout, J. McLaughlin, & Foucart, 2008; Hagoort et al, 1993; Osterhout & Mobley, 1995). Although some studies have reported an anterior negativity within a window ranging from 150 to 500 ms to some syntactic anomalies (the Left Anterior Negativity, or LAN: Friederici, 1995; Hahne & Friederici, 1999; Neville et al, 1991; Osterhout & Holcomb, 1992; Osterhout & Mobley, 1995), the P600 effect is more reliably correlated with syntactic manipulations.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The P600 is elicited by a number of typical morpho-syntactic violations, including violations of phrase-structure and subcategorization (e.g., Ainsworth-Darnell, Shulman, & Boland, 1998; Hagoort, Brown & Groothusen, 1993; Neville, Nicol, Barss, Forster, & Garrett, 1991; Osterhout & Holcomb, 1992) and number, gender, and case violations elicited in a variety of grammatical structures (Foucart & Frenck-Mestre, 2012; Frenck-Mestre, Foucart, Carrasco, & Herschensohn, 2009; Frenck-Mestre, Osterhout, McLaughlin & Foucart, 2008; Osterhout & Mobley, 1995). The P600 has generally been interpreted as signaling morpho-syntactic reanalysis and repair, suggesting a relatively late, non-automatic process.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent evidence suggests that even in related languages that both have grammatical gender, learning L2 gender is difficult (Lemhöfer, Schriefers & Hanique, 2010). Similar to research on gender agreement in syntactic contexts, in future research it will be important to investigate these lexical issues using measures that are particularly sensitive to the earliest time course of processing, such as ERPs (e.g., Frenck-Mestre, Foucart, Carrasco-Ortiz & Herschensohn, 2009; Foucart & Frenck-Mestre, 2012). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%