2014
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112543
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Producing Gestures Facilitates Route Learning

Abstract: The present study investigates whether producing gestures would facilitate route learning in a navigation task and whether its facilitation effect is comparable to that of hand movements that leave physical visible traces. In two experiments, we focused on gestures produced without accompanying speech, i.e., co-thought gestures (e.g., an index finger traces the spatial sequence of a route in the air). Adult participants were asked to study routes shown in four diagrams, one at a time. Participants reproduced t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
38
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
1
38
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Therefore, relative to actions, gesture forces people to create richer internal representations of the objects and to build stronger links between body movements and thinking. This account can explain why, in the Tower of Hanoi task, gesturing leads people to represent the weights of the disks, which are not relevant to the task, more strongly than producing the actions does (Beilock & GoldinMeadow, 2010; see also So et al, 2014 for a similar view). However, it is difficult for this account to explain gesture's advantage in generalizing strategies (Novack et al, 2014).…”
Section: Why Do Gestures Affect Thought More Than Actions?mentioning
confidence: 98%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Therefore, relative to actions, gesture forces people to create richer internal representations of the objects and to build stronger links between body movements and thinking. This account can explain why, in the Tower of Hanoi task, gesturing leads people to represent the weights of the disks, which are not relevant to the task, more strongly than producing the actions does (Beilock & GoldinMeadow, 2010; see also So et al, 2014 for a similar view). However, it is difficult for this account to explain gesture's advantage in generalizing strategies (Novack et al, 2014).…”
Section: Why Do Gestures Affect Thought More Than Actions?mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…This makes it more efficient to use the representations, as indicated by studies of route memory (So et al, 2014) and mental abacus (Hatano et al, 1977).…”
Section: Why Do Gestures Affect Thought More Than Actions?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…All PowerPoint files were then converted to video files. These routes were similar to those tested in So et al (2014). In each route, there were vertical, horizontal, and diagonal strokes, which were equal in length and connected to form a route navigating from the starting point to the destination (see Figure 1 for one of the 13-step routes).…”
Section: Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although previous study has established that producing gesture would facilitate encoding of spatial sequences (So et al, 2014), it is not clear whether the number of steps rehearsed in gestures (and spatial language, if it is found to be effective) would be positively related to recall performance. Previous research has shown that participants who were encouraged to gesture (gesture-encouraged condition) produced more gestures and performed better in the mental rotation task than those who were not encouraged to gesture but were allowed to move their hands (gesture-allowed condition; Chu & Kita, 2011).…”
mentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Furthermore, co-thought gestures are also consistently found to boost performance on other tasks, such as counting coins (Kirsh, 1995), mental abacus calculations (Brooks, 2014), tracking moving items in space (Delgado, Gómez, & Sarriá, 2011;Logan, Lowrie, & Diezmann, 2014;Macken & Ginns, 2014;So, Ching, Lim, Cheng, & Ip, 2014), solving fraction problems (Zurina & Williams, 2011), route learning (e.g., Logan et al, 2014;So et al, 2014), and rotating gear problems (e.g., Alibali, Spencer, Knox, & Kita, 2011;Stephen, Dixon, & Isenhower, 2009). The fact that these gestures also spontaneously occur without communicative intent suggests that, at least in some cases, gestures have a cognitive function that goes beyond supporting (communicative) speech processes (Pouw, de Nooijer, van Gog, Zwaan, & Paas, 2014;Pouw & Hostetter, 2016).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%