Interspeech 2004 2004
DOI: 10.21437/interspeech.2004-239
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prolongation in spontaneous Mandarin

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3
2
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 2 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This does not necessarily counter the idea that speakers generally produce prolongations before or on items with high cognitive load, e.g., either the preposition or article before a semantically heavy item [7], since verbs and nouns may as well precede semantically relevant elements such as a specific verb argument, or a noun's adjective (in Italian, qualifying adjectives mostly follow the noun they describe). Within word segments, prolongations most likely occur on word-final segments, i.e., 3-1-96%, resembling the distribution observed in Hebrew [13], Mandarin Chinese [9] and Japanese [8] and quite differently from those in Tok Pisin, German and from the somewhat more even distributions that characterize some Germanic languages, American English and Swedish [6], and Hungarian [10], see Table 3.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 87%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This does not necessarily counter the idea that speakers generally produce prolongations before or on items with high cognitive load, e.g., either the preposition or article before a semantically heavy item [7], since verbs and nouns may as well precede semantically relevant elements such as a specific verb argument, or a noun's adjective (in Italian, qualifying adjectives mostly follow the noun they describe). Within word segments, prolongations most likely occur on word-final segments, i.e., 3-1-96%, resembling the distribution observed in Hebrew [13], Mandarin Chinese [9] and Japanese [8] and quite differently from those in Tok Pisin, German and from the somewhat more even distributions that characterize some Germanic languages, American English and Swedish [6], and Hungarian [10], see Table 3.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…This study follows the framework set by the strand of corpus studies aimed at investigating and unveiling the role of prolongations as linguistic elements by comparing their use in different languages, ranging from Germanic languages (Swedish, American English [6], German [12]) to Tok Pisin [7], Chinese Mandarin [9], Hungarian [10] and Hebrew [13] and provides evidence from a Romance language, namely Italian.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Specifically, a filled pause is a semantically empty element of speech that delays the transfer of the speaker's message and is usually expressed in the form of "em", "uh", etc [7]. Prolongations are mainly used to indicate hesitation and to emphasize the discourse focus [10]. Since they both come after a character, we express them with an explicit label at the character level.…”
Section: Spontaneous Behavior Modeling and Predictingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, existing TTS systems are unable to provide sufficient performance and an immersive experience for spontaneous-style speech which typically occurs in conversations. Conversations often contain subtle spontaneous behaviors such as filled pause, prolongation, repair, repetition, laughing, coughing, etc., that make speech sound more genuine [7][8][9][10]. Hence, the modeling of spontaneous behaviors play a crucial role in spontaneous speech synthesis.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%