2001
DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.37.3.418
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prospective memory in children: The effects of age and task interruption.

Abstract: Prospective memory (PM), remembering to carry out a task in the future, is highly relevant to children's everyday functioning, yet relatively little is known about it. For these reasons the effects of age and task interruption on PM were studied in 3 experiments. Children, aged 4-, 5-, and 7-years, were asked to name pictures in stacks of cards (the ongoing task) and to remember to do something when they saw a target picture (the PM task). Significant age differences were identified, but age explained only a s… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
199
3
2

Year Published

2010
2010
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 166 publications
(214 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
10
199
3
2
Order By: Relevance
“…For the 7-9-year olds we found a similar amount of prospective and retrospective memory complaints, while the 11-12-year olds were judged to commit more prospective than retrospective memory failures. The findings for the 7-9-year olds are in line with previous data of a greater decrease of reported retrospective as compared to prospective memory problems with age in 2-6-year old children (Kliegel & Jäger, 2007) and of greater performance decrements in retrospective as compared to prospective memory tasks in children this age (Kvavilashvili et al, 2001). It seems that for children aged 11 to 12 years like in adults, parents regard prospective memory failures as more frequent than retrospective memory problems.…”
Section: Memory Complaints Across Lifespan 21supporting
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For the 7-9-year olds we found a similar amount of prospective and retrospective memory complaints, while the 11-12-year olds were judged to commit more prospective than retrospective memory failures. The findings for the 7-9-year olds are in line with previous data of a greater decrease of reported retrospective as compared to prospective memory problems with age in 2-6-year old children (Kliegel & Jäger, 2007) and of greater performance decrements in retrospective as compared to prospective memory tasks in children this age (Kvavilashvili et al, 2001). It seems that for children aged 11 to 12 years like in adults, parents regard prospective memory failures as more frequent than retrospective memory problems.…”
Section: Memory Complaints Across Lifespan 21supporting
confidence: 81%
“…However, in comparison to prospective memory, retrospective memory performance increases in young children (Kvavilashvili et al, 2001) respectively decrements in old adults seem to be more pronounced (Henry, MacLeod, Phillips, & Crawford, 2004;Kvavilashvili et al, 2009). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Detailed description and results of IQ and EF used at one year follow-up can be found elsewhere 21 . Most individuals (26) were still children at the time of the study but 13 had become young adults. Thirty-seven controls were recruited (two controls could not be recruited within the time frame of the study).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As evidence of this claim, Kvavilashvili, Messer, and Ebdon (2001) report that PM failures represent 50% to 70% of everyday memory problems (see also Crovitz & Daniel, 1984;Terry, 1988). From a developmental perspective, the ability to carry out an intended activity is a critical skill that children must acquire as they gain independence from their caregivers (Kvavilashvili et al, 2001).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%