2013
DOI: 10.4074/s0003503313003059
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Questioning the acceptability of the Cognitive Interview to improve its use

Abstract: Introduced in the middle of the 1980s, the cognitive interview intended to perfect the techniques of interviewing witnesses and victims of crimes and offences. Since then, numerous international researchers have found an interest in this technique. The major reason for its success within the scientific community is probably due to the scientific basis of its elaboration, referring to various models of memory. Among the potential users-mainly professionals of justice-its use is not systematic, even for those wi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 105 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, our use of a 3-AFC test is likely similar to many of the direct questioning methods used on eyewitnesses. Although there are many benefits to using free-recall type questioning in forensic settings such as the cognitive interview (Fisher & Geiselman, 1992), we note that this type of questioning is not practiced universally (e.g., Brunel & Py, 2013; Wells, Memon, & Penrod, 2006) and may leave witnesses with tests/questioning procedures that require discrimination of correct and incorrect information like the 3-AFC. Thus, our paradigm shares many similarities with eyewitness events and laboratory-based paradigms and even offers advantages of using names and locations as misinformation which are often critical details.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Finally, our use of a 3-AFC test is likely similar to many of the direct questioning methods used on eyewitnesses. Although there are many benefits to using free-recall type questioning in forensic settings such as the cognitive interview (Fisher & Geiselman, 1992), we note that this type of questioning is not practiced universally (e.g., Brunel & Py, 2013; Wells, Memon, & Penrod, 2006) and may leave witnesses with tests/questioning procedures that require discrimination of correct and incorrect information like the 3-AFC. Thus, our paradigm shares many similarities with eyewitness events and laboratory-based paradigms and even offers advantages of using names and locations as misinformation which are often critical details.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…More precisely, the improvement in knowledge that results from the training, does not necessarily translate into practice (e.g., Aldridge & Cameron, 1999;Stevenson, Leung, & Cheung, 1992;Warren et al, 1999). In order to explore this issue further and perhaps make the training sessions more efficient, a future direction of research could be to explore the acceptability of the CI techniques (Brunel & Py, 2013), as the acceptability of innovations or of behavioral changes is known to be linked with their application. Thirdly, our hypotheses concerning potential differences between the partial MCIs relied on arguments concerning changes in report criteria.…”
Section: Limitations and Perspectivesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This question is important from an applied perspective, given that free recall shares similar characteristics with the cognitive interview used in forensic settings (Fisher & Geiselman, 1992). Asking eyewitnesses to begin their accounts with free recall may thus benefit memory accuracy (Wilford et al, 2014), even though this procedure is not universally used in practice (Brunel & Py, 2013; Wells, Memon, & Penrod, 2006). Finding a PET in free recall would broaden the evidence that initial testing sometimes improves memory accuracy and thus would provide further incentive for studying the application of initial free recall techniques in forensic settings.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%