2011
DOI: 10.1007/s11049-011-9148-y
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reaching agreement

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
36
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
0
36
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Meaning has been hypothesized to affect agreement computation by affecting the conceptual number of individual words as well as the subject NP as a whole (see, e.g., Bock & Middleton, 2011; Eberhard et al, 2005; and references therein), and the variety of semantically-rich content verbs used in previous RC conditions may have produced differences in conceptual number which reduced RC mismatch effects. For example, RCs with semantically-rich content verbs may be more likely to be interpreted as restrictive than PPs or RCs with less semantically-specific verbs (like those in Experiments 1 and 2).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Meaning has been hypothesized to affect agreement computation by affecting the conceptual number of individual words as well as the subject NP as a whole (see, e.g., Bock & Middleton, 2011; Eberhard et al, 2005; and references therein), and the variety of semantically-rich content verbs used in previous RC conditions may have produced differences in conceptual number which reduced RC mismatch effects. For example, RCs with semantically-rich content verbs may be more likely to be interpreted as restrictive than PPs or RCs with less semantically-specific verbs (like those in Experiments 1 and 2).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Staub (2009) argued that these findings supported the notion that the ambiguously-valued global number representation (e.g., a ‘slightly plural’ S(r) value) gives rise to uncertainty about the correct verb form, which in turn leads to longer RTs. Thus, in addition to accurately predicting effects on attraction rates of notional number (e.g., Bock, Carreiras, & Meseguer, 2012; Brehm & Bock, 2013), morphological richness (e.g., Foote & Bock, 2012; Lorimor, Bock, Zalkind, Sheyman, & Beard, 2008), and other factors (see Bock & Middleton, 2011, for a recent review), the MM model also correctly predicts symmetrical attraction effects during language production.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…*the key to the cabinets were missing; e.g. Bock 2001;Bock & Middleton, 2011;Eberhard et al, 2005). Once again, further specification may allow predictions to be made and tested regarding patterns of behaviour that are more or less likely given the architecture and processing assumptions proposed here.…”
Section: Connected Speechmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Paucal equates to 'few' in English, and indeed, while it is not grammatically marked in English, there may nevertheless be a PAUCAL conceptual node which would activate the determiner 'few' (see below). Bock and Middleton (2011) note that there is a perceptual basis for the conceptual and grammatical coding of number that lies in our perceptual capacity to automatically enumerate between one and three objects. With more than three objects, judgments of number depend on counting, although judgments of relative quantity still occur automatically in terms of approximate magnitudes (Hyde & Spelke, 2009).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%