2014
DOI: 10.1177/0255761413515813
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reading ahead: Adult music students’ eye movements in temporally controlled performances of a children’s song

Abstract: In the present study, education majors minoring in music education (n = 24) and music performance majors (n =14) read and performed the original version and melodically altered versions of a simple melody in a given tempo. Eye movements during music reading and piano performances were recorded. Errorless trials were analyzed to explore the adjustments of visual processing in successful performances. The temporal length of the eye–hand span (time between gaze and the performed note) was typically around one sec… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
99
0
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(105 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
5
99
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Participants who played faster were regarded as skilled sight-readers, and conclusions, such as that skilled sight-readers (equal to faster players) had a longer EHS, were drawn 8,1116 . However, as already indicated in previous studies 10,17–19 , a problem associated with uncontrolled playing tempo across participants is that the EHS is supposedly longer when playing the same length of a sight-reading piece at a faster tempo. For example, Huovinen et al .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…Participants who played faster were regarded as skilled sight-readers, and conclusions, such as that skilled sight-readers (equal to faster players) had a longer EHS, were drawn 8,1116 . However, as already indicated in previous studies 10,17–19 , a problem associated with uncontrolled playing tempo across participants is that the EHS is supposedly longer when playing the same length of a sight-reading piece at a faster tempo. For example, Huovinen et al .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…This expertise effect has been noticed both in silent-reading tasks (Waters & Underwood, 1998;Penttinen, et al, 2013) and during-performance tasks. Considering the latter, experts' advanced skill has appeared both when more and less experienced musicians' performances differ in terms of performance tempo (Truitt, Clifton, Pollatsek, & Rayner, 1997;Gilman & Underwood, 2003;Penttinen & Huovinen, 2011) but also when all performances are alike in a temporal sense and in performance quality (Penttinen, Huovinen, & Ylitalo, 2015). The features of the music notation, too, play a role in the reading, though this side of the process has only rarely been studied and when it has been, often only on a very general level (Madell & Hébert, 2008;Puurtinen, 2018).…”
Section: Why Use Eye Tracking In the Music Domain?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, musicians have to make sense of the complex symbolic system and select what to target their gaze at, as well as when and for how long, in order to perform successfully while operating within the given temporal framework. We know that musicians manage this by maintaining their gaze slightly ahead of the current point of performance, and with the help of this buffer (typically of perhaps around 1-2 seconds [Furneaux & Land, 1999;Penttinen, et al, 2015;Huovinen, Ylitalo, & Puurtinen, 2018;Rosemann, Altenmüller, & Fahle, 2016; see Video 1]), the performer prepares for the upcoming motoric responses. The gaze may typically tend to remain very close to the performed notes (Truitt et al, 1997;Penttinen, et al, 2015), but instead of a steady "looking ahead", at least for skilled music readers, the reading may also consist (mainly or in part) of rapid back and forth eye movements (Goolsby, 1994;cf.…”
Section: Music Reading Is All About the Use Of Timementioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations