1995
DOI: 10.1016/0926-6410(95)00018-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reading habits and line bisection: a developmental approach

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
72
1
1

Year Published

2002
2002
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 134 publications
(81 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
7
72
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This bias disappeared when the participants had been trained in the right-to-left direction (the subjective midpoint nearly coincided with the true midpoint in this case, regardless of the side of presentation). The main effect of training was significant [F (1,8) Chokron and Imbert (1993) and Chokron and de Agostini (1995), who demonstrated the existence of a rightward bias in the estimation of the subjective midpoint by monolingual Arabic and Israeli readers tested using the classical bisection task. However, remember that the participants in the present experiment were bilingual students (with a possible bias in favor of left-to-right reading), whereas Chokron and colleagues' participantswere monolinguals.This can probably explain why a lack of bias (instead of the expected reversed bias) was observed here for the right-to-left training condition.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This bias disappeared when the participants had been trained in the right-to-left direction (the subjective midpoint nearly coincided with the true midpoint in this case, regardless of the side of presentation). The main effect of training was significant [F (1,8) Chokron and Imbert (1993) and Chokron and de Agostini (1995), who demonstrated the existence of a rightward bias in the estimation of the subjective midpoint by monolingual Arabic and Israeli readers tested using the classical bisection task. However, remember that the participants in the present experiment were bilingual students (with a possible bias in favor of left-to-right reading), whereas Chokron and colleagues' participantswere monolinguals.This can probably explain why a lack of bias (instead of the expected reversed bias) was observed here for the right-to-left training condition.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, people are quicker to identify the names of powerful groups in the upper part of the visual field and powerless groups in the lower part of the visual field (Schubert, 2005). Third, people imagine action in the left-right axis more readily when it proceeds in the direction of their written language; from left-to-right for English and Italian speakers and from right-to-left for Arabic speakers, for example (Chatterjee, Southwood, & Basilico, 1999;Maass, Pagani, & Berta, 2007;Maass & Russo, 2003; see also Chokron & De Agostini, 1996Spalek & Hammad, 2005). Recently, Maass, Suitner, Favaretto, and Cignacchi (2009) have shown that imagery along the left-right axis is further affected by stereotypes about groups' agency.…”
Section: Alternative Explanations Of Graph Order Preferencesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Reading habits influence people's preferences, decision making, perceptual-motor performance, attentional momentum, and how they represent numbers and time (e.g., Chokron & De Agostini, 1995, 2000Eviator, 1995;Shaki, Fischer, & Petrusic, 2009;Spalek & Hammad, 2005;Van der Henst & Schaeken, 2005). In most of the studies listed here, the effects of reading habits on cognition were examined using cross-cultural comparisons of readers with a left-to-right (e.g., English) orthographic system to readers with a right-to-left (e.g., Arabic) orthographic system.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%