Humans appear to rely on spatial mappings to describe and represent concepts. In particular, conceptual cueing refers to the effect whereby after reading or hearing a particular word, the location of observers' visual attention in space can be systematically shifted in a particular direction. For example, words such as Bsun^and Bhappy^orient attention upwards, whereas words such as Bbasement^and Bbitter^ori-ent attention downwards. This area of research has garnered much interest, particularly within the embodied cognition framework, for its potential to enhance our understanding of the interaction between abstract cognitive processes such as language and basic visual processes such as attention and stimulus processing. To date, however, this area has relied on subjective classification criteria to determine whether words ought to be classified as having a meaning that implies Bup^or Bdown.^The present study, therefore, provides a set of 498 items that have each been systematically rated by over 90 participants, providing refined, continuous measures of the extent to which people associate given words with particular spatial dimensions. The resulting database provides an objective means to aid item-selection for future research in this area.Keywords Language . Metaphor . Visual attention . Cueing . Conceptual cueing . Conceptual metaphor . Embodied cognition . Embodied semantics There is considerable interaction between abstract cognitive processes on the one hand and one's body and external environment on the other. For example, memory is dramatically enhanced by mentally representing content in familiar spatial locations (Maguire, Valentine, Wilding, & Kapur, 2003), insight problem solving is facilitated by prescribed physical movements (Thomas & Lleras, 2007, 2009, and visual awareness of an object is affected by proximity of the observer's hands (Goodhew, Edwards, Ferber, & Pratt, 2015;Goodhew, Gozli, Ferber, & Pratt, 2013). Furthermore, humans appear to draw on concrete spatial layouts in order to describe and represent concepts (e.g., Boroditsky, Fuhrman, & McCormick, 2011). For example, English speakers describe someone who is sad as down, describe improvement as things looking up, and we look forward to the future or back to the past.A growing body of studies documents the entwined relationship between concepts and space, in particular, how activating word meaning can systematically shift visual attention in space (e.g., Ansorge, Khalid, & Konig, 2013;Chasteen, Burdzy, & Pratt, 2010;Dudschig, De la Vega, & Kaup, 2015;Dudschig, Souman, Lachmair, de la Vega, & Kaup, 2013;Estes, Verges, & Barsalou, 2008;Fischer, Castel, Dodd, & Pratt, 2003; Gozli, Chow, Chasteen, & Pratt, 2013;Louwerse & Jeuniaux, 2010;Meier & Robinson, 2004;Santiago, Lupianez, Perez, & Funes, 2007;Setic & Domijan, 2007;Weger & Pratt, 2008;Zwaan & Yaxley, 2003). For example, after reading a word associated with up (such as Bsun^or Bjoy^), participants are faster to respond to subsequent visual targets above the center of the screen...