2014
DOI: 10.1167/14.1.2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Real and implied motion at the center of gaze

Abstract: Even though the dynamicity of our environment is a given, much of what we know on fixation selection comes from studies of static scene viewing. We performed a direct comparison of fixation selection on static and dynamic visual stimuli and investigated how far identical mechanisms drive these. We recorded eye movements while participants viewed movie clips of natural scenery and static frames taken from the same movies. Both were presented in the same high spatial resolution (1080 × 1920 pixels). The static c… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 124 publications
0
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Kourtzi et al [9] found greater activation of the medial temporal/medial superior temporal cortex (MT/MST) when viewing photographs with implied movement and imaging studies have supported the role of these brain areas in the analysis of movement, but not object, recognition [28]. Action understanding depends, in part, on prior knowledge about the movement’s goal and intention with predictions about an object’s (or body’s) future position being made from the motion implied in the static image [29]. Participant’s memorial biases modulate the increase in attentional bias to threatening information and, therefore, they perceive implied painful motion in the image (e.g., rotation of the back, which causes them pain); the dynamic images accessing a more meaningful motor representation that is presented for analysis through the variety of eye gaze metrics employed in this study.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Kourtzi et al [9] found greater activation of the medial temporal/medial superior temporal cortex (MT/MST) when viewing photographs with implied movement and imaging studies have supported the role of these brain areas in the analysis of movement, but not object, recognition [28]. Action understanding depends, in part, on prior knowledge about the movement’s goal and intention with predictions about an object’s (or body’s) future position being made from the motion implied in the static image [29]. Participant’s memorial biases modulate the increase in attentional bias to threatening information and, therefore, they perceive implied painful motion in the image (e.g., rotation of the back, which causes them pain); the dynamic images accessing a more meaningful motor representation that is presented for analysis through the variety of eye gaze metrics employed in this study.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Apparently, the applied approach to calculate ES was not able to identify the target area of Global motion. Alternative salience computations are available to calculate, for example, motion-directed contrast and boundaries (Açik, Bartel, & König, 2014;Black & Anandan, 1996).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In other words, 'STOP' primed the stop goal in only in conditions in which the goal was relevant to the task context. Note that Chiu and Aron have shown that the effects of lowvisibility primes may also be context-dependent (Chiu & Aron, 2014; but see Lin and Murray (2014), who raised some methodological concerns about their priming manipulation). Finally, contingent involuntary response inhibition was also demonstrated by Folk (2012, 2014), who found that flankers that share the colour of a no-go stimulus could suppress motor responses.…”
Section: Inhibition As a Primed Or Prepared Reflexmentioning
confidence: 99%