2000
DOI: 10.1016/s0389-4304(00)00067-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Rear-end collision scenarios categorized by type of human error

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
3
0
3

Year Published

2009
2009
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
3
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…This suggests that the frequency of rear-end conflicts in the simulation increases in accordance with traffic volume on the signaled approaches. This finding is consistent with real-world situations where increased traffic volume implies shorter time gaps on average between vehicles and where shorter gaps increase the likelihood for rear-end collisions (18). Table 2 also shows the number of red light violations in each of the two simulation scenarios.…”
Section: Results Of Simulating the Ir Functionsupporting
confidence: 82%
“…This suggests that the frequency of rear-end conflicts in the simulation increases in accordance with traffic volume on the signaled approaches. This finding is consistent with real-world situations where increased traffic volume implies shorter time gaps on average between vehicles and where shorter gaps increase the likelihood for rear-end collisions (18). Table 2 also shows the number of red light violations in each of the two simulation scenarios.…”
Section: Results Of Simulating the Ir Functionsupporting
confidence: 82%
“…Driving task/road scenario: Two different driving tasks/road scenarios have been selected as the most applicable for the selected research hypotheses, which are namely the free driving scenario and the following vehicle scenario. The following vehicle driving task has been selected because it has been recognized as a major road safety issue [30,36,38], mainly due to its close relation to the often catastrophic rear-end collisions, while it is important that both driving tasks, due to them being quite generic, lacking specific measures imposition, are optimum for allowing the collection of comparative data which would serve as horizontal "exposure data" for other, more sophisticated driving tasks/scenarios. 3.…”
Section: Experimental Plan Variablesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Significant research has been conducted so far on driving simulator validity and there are various attempts to correlate the simulator results with those being extracted from trials in real traffic conditions. Relevant research started since the mid 1960's with Barrett et al (1965) and is continuing until today, with Godley et al (2001), Yu et al (2006), Kim et al (2006), Miyajima et al (2006), Hirata et al (2007), de Winter et al (2009), Riener (2010), etc. Among them, it is worth mentioning Jia et al (2011), who moved one step further, and they suggested an approach for their simulator calibration through a correlation model, relying on measurements from trials in real traffic conditions.…”
Section: Executive Summarymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Έρεη πξαγκαηνπνηεζεί πιεζψξα εξεπλψλ ζρεηηθά κε ηελ αμηνπηζηία ησλ πξνζνκνησηψλ νδήγεζεο θαη είλαη πνηθίιεο νη πξνζπάζεηεο ζπζρέηηζεο ησλ απνηειεζκάησλ απφ πξνζνκνησηή νδήγεζεο κε απηά απφ δνθηκέο ζε πξαγκαηηθέο ζπλζήθεο. Ζ ζρεηηθή έξεπλα μεθίλεζε απφ ηα κέζα ηεο δεθαεηίαο ηνπ 1960 κε ηνπο Barrett et al (1965) θαη εθηείλεηαη κέρξη ηηο κέξεο καο κε ηνπο Godley et al (2001), Yu et al (2006), Kim et al (2006), Miyajima et al (2006), Hirata et al (2007), de Winter et al (2009), Riener (2010), θ.α. Αλάκεζά ηνπο, μερσξίδνπλ νη Jia et al (2011), πνπ πξνρψξεζαλ έλα βήκα παξαπέξα, θαη πξφηεηλαλ κία πξνζέγγηζε ξχζκηζεο/βαζκνλφκεζεο ηνπ πξνζνκνησηή ηνπο κέζσ ελφο κνληέινπ ζπζρέηηζεο, κε βάζε ηα απνηειέζκαηα απφ δνθηκέο ζε πξαγκαηηθέο ζπλζήθεο.…”
unclassified
See 1 more Smart Citation