2018
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/8gjth
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reasoning supports forgiving accidental harms

Abstract: People experience a strong conflict while condemning someone who brought about an accidental harm, her innocent intention exonerating her, but the harmful outcome incriminating her. In the present research (total N = 4879), we explore how reasoning ability and cognitive style relate to how people choose to resolve this conflict and judge the accidental harms. A first set of studies (1a-c) showed that individual differences in cognitive style predicted severity of judgments in fictitious accidental harms scenar… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
5
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
2

Relationship

2
0

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
(71 reference statements)
1
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Given that we were interested in the effect of both intent and harm severity on moral evaluations and their interaction, we ran the power analysis on the interaction. We used a medium effect size based on a prior data set which found a medium effect of reasoning on moral judgement of accidental harms ( β = –0.15); see Patil & Trémolière, 2021. We set an effect size f = 0.25 (medium effect size) with numerator df = 1 (2 predictors with 2 levels each), and number of groups = 4 (corresponding to the 4 possible combinations of intention by harm severity that were presented to each participant).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Given that we were interested in the effect of both intent and harm severity on moral evaluations and their interaction, we ran the power analysis on the interaction. We used a medium effect size based on a prior data set which found a medium effect of reasoning on moral judgement of accidental harms ( β = –0.15); see Patil & Trémolière, 2021. We set an effect size f = 0.25 (medium effect size) with numerator df = 1 (2 predictors with 2 levels each), and number of groups = 4 (corresponding to the 4 possible combinations of intention by harm severity that were presented to each participant).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The stories framed an interaction between an agent and a victim in a daily-life context, ending with the perpetrator harming the victim. The perpetrator’s intention was not explicitly stated but had to be inferred by participants from the narratives (see also Patil & Trémolière, 2021). Letting participants infer intentions is indeed more in line with real-life interactions where people have to attribute mental states to others based on implicit cues.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Several prior studies explore the role of reasoning in utilitarian moral judgment by taxing executive resources necessary for deliberation (Patil & Trémolière, 2018;Trémolière, Neys, & Bonnefon, 2018). This is done in several ways: a) time pressure manipulation: limiting the amount of time available to provide moral judgments (Cummins & Cummins, 2012;Rosas & Aguilar-Pardo, 2019;Suter & Hertwig, 2011); b) cognitive load manipulation: taxing working memory capacity through another concurrent cognitively demanding task (Conway & Gawronski, 2013;Gawronski, Armstrong, Conway, Friesdorf, & Hütter, 2017;Greene, Morelli, Lowenberg, Nystrom, & Cohen, 2008), or exhausting cognitive resources via sleep deprivation (Killgore et al, 2007;Tempesta et al, 2012) or with a prior sequential cognitive depletion task (Timmons & Byrne, 2018), or leading people to think about their mortality (Trémolière, Neys, & Bonnefon, 2012), or, the reverse, easing up the cognitive load by presenting efficient kill-save ratios (Trémolière & Bonnefon, 2014); c) cognitive priming manipulation: nudging participants to use a deliberative thinking mode (versus "feelings thinking mode") (Li, Xia, Wu, & Chen, 2018), priming analytical thinking mode by asking them to solve mathematical puzzles before performing the moral judgment task (Kvaran, Nichols, & Sanfey, 2013), or presenting dilemmas written in hard-to-read (disfluent) fonts (Spears, Fernández-Linsenbarth, Okan, Ruz, & González, 2018) to trigger analytic thinking.…”
Section: Manipulations Of Reasoningmentioning
confidence: 99%