“…Several prior studies explore the role of reasoning in utilitarian moral judgment by taxing executive resources necessary for deliberation (Patil & Trémolière, 2018;Trémolière, Neys, & Bonnefon, 2018). This is done in several ways: a) time pressure manipulation: limiting the amount of time available to provide moral judgments (Cummins & Cummins, 2012;Rosas & Aguilar-Pardo, 2019;Suter & Hertwig, 2011); b) cognitive load manipulation: taxing working memory capacity through another concurrent cognitively demanding task (Conway & Gawronski, 2013;Gawronski, Armstrong, Conway, Friesdorf, & Hütter, 2017;Greene, Morelli, Lowenberg, Nystrom, & Cohen, 2008), or exhausting cognitive resources via sleep deprivation (Killgore et al, 2007;Tempesta et al, 2012) or with a prior sequential cognitive depletion task (Timmons & Byrne, 2018), or leading people to think about their mortality (Trémolière, Neys, & Bonnefon, 2012), or, the reverse, easing up the cognitive load by presenting efficient kill-save ratios (Trémolière & Bonnefon, 2014); c) cognitive priming manipulation: nudging participants to use a deliberative thinking mode (versus "feelings thinking mode") (Li, Xia, Wu, & Chen, 2018), priming analytical thinking mode by asking them to solve mathematical puzzles before performing the moral judgment task (Kvaran, Nichols, & Sanfey, 2013), or presenting dilemmas written in hard-to-read (disfluent) fonts (Spears, Fernández-Linsenbarth, Okan, Ruz, & González, 2018) to trigger analytic thinking.…”