2010
DOI: 10.3758/app.72.6.1444
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Recognizing famous people

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

7
44
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 48 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
7
44
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Figure 2(e) indicates also that the mouth region was not a particularly salient cue for these particular stimuli. Nevertheless, all three participant groups relied significantly on information in this region (as also found previously Gosselin & Schyns, 2001; though see Butler et al, 2010). The use of these less than salient cues in the typical children and adults suggests that a bias to sub-optimally encode redundant facial features is not unique to autism.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 81%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Figure 2(e) indicates also that the mouth region was not a particularly salient cue for these particular stimuli. Nevertheless, all three participant groups relied significantly on information in this region (as also found previously Gosselin & Schyns, 2001; though see Butler et al, 2010). The use of these less than salient cues in the typical children and adults suggests that a bias to sub-optimally encode redundant facial features is not unique to autism.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 81%
“…Here, they relied significantly upon information in the eye region (particularly the left side eye) and the mouth region, as reported in Butler et al (2010), Caldara et al (2005), Schyns et al (2002). Small idiosyncrasies were observed between these two typical groups, for example, adults consistently also used a left side jawline cue.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 86%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These findings raise the question of how such fast and preconscious processing can be achieved—through a subcortical system (for a review see Tamietto and de Gelder, 2010 but see also Pessoa and Adolphs, 2010) or through a cortical route with a fast feed-forward integration of information (VanRullen and Thorpe, 2001) and activation of the distributed network in the fronto-parietal areas for retrieval of person knowledge. Highly-learned representations of personally familiar faces may also include detectors for visual features—face fragments or more holistic configurations—that are diagnostic for familiar individuals (Butler et al, 2010). The facilitation of familiar face processing that appears to be at least partially independent of attentional resources and awareness may be due to activation of such learned diagnostic feature detectors.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Oxytocin has also shown to enhance the ability to infer the mental state of others on a task that requires sensitivity to subtle information from the eye region (Baron-Cohen et al, 2001; Domes et al, 2007). This is relevant to prosopagnosia in that the eye region is highly diagnostic for face recognition (Butler et al, 2010) and that processing of the eye region has been shown to be particularly impaired in prosopagnosics (DeGutis et al, 2012b). Further supporting this link between oxytocin and facial recognition ability, a recent study of 178 families with at least one autistic child found that variation in the oxytocin receptor gene, OXTR , was strongly associated with face recognition performance on the Warrington Face Memory Test (Skuse et al, 2014).…”
Section: Other Treatment Approaches In Developmental Prosopagnosiamentioning
confidence: 99%