Three experiments explored the consistency effect in word and nonword pronunciation. All three experiments involved a common second phase in which subjects were required to pronounce a sequence of consistent and inconsistent words and nonwords. The experiments were distinguished by the nature of Phase 1. In Experiment 1, subjects pronounced a sequence of words and nonwords in which all the words were exceptions. In Experiment 2, all the items in this phase were consistent, and in Experiment 3, they were inconsistent. Consistency effects for words, as measured by pronunciation latency, were confined to Experiment 1. Consistency effects for nonwords were found in Experiment 2. No consistency effects were found for either words or nonwords in Experiment 3. There was clear evidence of a consistency effect for nonwords in Experiment 1 in terms of pronunciation errors, but the latency data were not significant. This is attributed to the very slow response times in this condition. All these data are compatible with both revised analogy and dual-route theories.Two classes of theory seek to explain the processes whereby the pronunciation of words and nonwords is achieved: the dual-route theory and the analogy theory. The former, as originally proposed by Coltheart (1978) and subsequently developed by Morton and Patterson (1980) and Parkin (1984), postulates two functionally independent methods of word processing. One of these is a lexical processing route that operates by the direct mapping of the visual characteristics of a letter string onto its stored lexical representation in the mental lexicon. In addition, a nonlexical processing route is assumed to translate the visual code of a word into a phonological one on the basis of spelling-to-sound rules. Coltheart proposed that this phonemic transcription operated by means of grapheme-to-phoneme correspondence rules (GPCs) as specified by Venezky (1970). These rules were derived by considering a maximum of two letters at a time (Le., vowel, vowel digraph, consonant, and consonant clusters) to define their major and minor correspondences. For example, the major correspondence (Le., the most common pronunciation) of the vowel digraph EA is Ii! as in BEAN, and minor Oess common) ones are leil as in BREAK and lei as in BREAD. Thus the phonemic transcription of the word BEACH would yield fbi, Iii, and ItJI. ' Recent modifications to dual-route theory have amended Coltheart's (1978) assertion that the nonlexical route operates solely by the application of GPCs. Shallice, Warrington, and McCarthy (1983) and Parkin (1984) suggested that nonlexical assembly of phonology may also use larger patterns of letters operating beyond the GPC level. If this is the case, some words exhibiting a minor correspondence in terms of Venezky's (1970) rules have a major correWe are grateful to the editor and to two anonymous reviewers for their many helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. Please direct all correspondence to: Alan J. Parkin, Laboratory of Experimental Psychology,...