2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.062
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reduced Laughter Contagion in Boys at Risk for Psychopathy

Abstract: SummaryHumans are intrinsically social animals, forming enduring affiliative bonds [1]. However, a striking minority with psychopathic traits, who present with violent and antisocial behaviors, tend to value other people only insofar as they contribute to their own advancement [2, 3]. Extant research has addressed the neurocognitive processes associated with aggression in such individuals, but we know remarkably little about processes underlying their atypical social affiliation. This is surprising, given the … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
65
2

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 51 publications
(74 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
7
65
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The robustness of our findings is supported by their consistency with similar prior work (Decety, Chen, et al, 2013;Decety, Skelly, & Kiehl, 2013;Lockwood et al, 2013;Marsh et al, 2013;Meffert et al, 2013;Seara-Cardoso, Viding, Lickley, & Sebastian, 2015) but should be confirmed in larger sample sizes. It is important to note, the extent to which our findings would generalize to a wider array of affective outcomes, including empathy for positive emotional states (recently suggested to be disrupted as well in psychopathy [O'Nions et al, 2017]), cannot be determined from this study because we considered responses only to painful stimuli in the present study. This increased the specificity of our findings, as the nature of self-other neural mapping implies that distinct patterns of activation should be observed for distinct affective and sensory states.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…The robustness of our findings is supported by their consistency with similar prior work (Decety, Chen, et al, 2013;Decety, Skelly, & Kiehl, 2013;Lockwood et al, 2013;Marsh et al, 2013;Meffert et al, 2013;Seara-Cardoso, Viding, Lickley, & Sebastian, 2015) but should be confirmed in larger sample sizes. It is important to note, the extent to which our findings would generalize to a wider array of affective outcomes, including empathy for positive emotional states (recently suggested to be disrupted as well in psychopathy [O'Nions et al, 2017]), cannot be determined from this study because we considered responses only to painful stimuli in the present study. This increased the specificity of our findings, as the nature of self-other neural mapping implies that distinct patterns of activation should be observed for distinct affective and sensory states.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…5,42 Laughter: A recent study by our research group is the only investigation to date to focus on how children at risk of developing psychopathy process genuine laughter. 44 We asked the children to listen to clips of genuine laughter in the scanner. After the scanning session they listened to the laugher stimuli again and reported their desire to join in with the laughter.…”
Section: Psychopathy: Cognitive and Neural Function In Domains Importmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Meta-analyses of 26 studies found emotional recognition of facial expressions and vocal cues was significantly impaired in young and adult psychopaths for all of the basic emotions: anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise ( Dawel et al, 2012 ). Their inability to recognize fear and sadness was especially pronounced, but they also exhibit reduced neural response to laughter ( O’Nions et al, 2017 ). Such blunted emotions affect perceptions, thought processes and actions toward others, fostering both boldness and lack of remorse.…”
Section: Neurobiology Of Psychopathymentioning
confidence: 99%