2016
DOI: 10.1002/jaba.363
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reducing overselective stimulus control with differential observing responses

Abstract: Overselective stimulus control refers to discriminative control in which the number of controlling stimuli is too limited for effective behavior. Experiment 1 included 22 special-education students who exhibited overselective stimulus control on a two-sample delayed matching task. An intervention added a compound identity matching opportunity within the sample observation period of the matching trials. The compound matching functioned as a differential observing response (DOR) in that high accuracy verified ob… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
37
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
0
37
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Grow and LeBlanc (2013) described practice recommendations for conducting conditional discrimination training with children with autism that might minimize the likelihood of faulty stimulus control (Sidman, 1992) that sometimes occurs with simple to conditional discrimination training, particularly for the earliest learners. Many of the recommendations were based on the experimental literature on stimulus control (Green, 2001), and each recommendation has subsequently been demonstrated and replicated either in isolation or in some combination in applied studies with children with autism (Farber, Dickson, & Dube, 2017;Fisher, Retzlaff, Akers, DeSouza & Kaminski, 2019;Grow, Carr, Kodak, Jostad & Kisamore, 2011;Grow, Kodak & Carr, 2014;Vedora & Grandeliski, 2015). One recommendation in particular involved using a specially designed datasheet to provide the preset target stimulus and a counterbalanced three-item array of comparison stimuli for each trial.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Grow and LeBlanc (2013) described practice recommendations for conducting conditional discrimination training with children with autism that might minimize the likelihood of faulty stimulus control (Sidman, 1992) that sometimes occurs with simple to conditional discrimination training, particularly for the earliest learners. Many of the recommendations were based on the experimental literature on stimulus control (Green, 2001), and each recommendation has subsequently been demonstrated and replicated either in isolation or in some combination in applied studies with children with autism (Farber, Dickson, & Dube, 2017;Fisher, Retzlaff, Akers, DeSouza & Kaminski, 2019;Grow, Carr, Kodak, Jostad & Kisamore, 2011;Grow, Kodak & Carr, 2014;Vedora & Grandeliski, 2015). One recommendation in particular involved using a specially designed datasheet to provide the preset target stimulus and a counterbalanced three-item array of comparison stimuli for each trial.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is fortunate that stimulus overselectivity and blocking may be reduced by teaching an overt precurrent ("observing") response to the S + (Doughty & Hopkins, 2011), as well as by teaching conditional discrimination from the beginning of a procedure, rather than following a simple discrimination (Green, 2001). Moreover, Farber et al (2017) suggested that differential observing responses (e.g., different tacts to sample stimuli) during matching-to-sample resulted in less overselectivity than nondifferential observing responses, where the response to the sample is the same on every trial, in children with autism (e.g., Reed, Altweck, Broomfield, Simpson, & McHugh, 2012). Differential observing responses verify the discrimination of stimulus features that differ among the samples (e.g., by tacting the sample stimuli).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Researchers have suggested that blocking is likely related to stimulus overselectivity, in which some relevant properties of a compound stimulus fail to acquire stimulus control (e.g., Cengher, Budd, Farrell, & Fienup, 2018;Cipani, 2012;Farber, Dickson, & Dube, 2017;Lovaas, Schreibman, Koegel, & Rehm, 1971;Ploog, 2010). Such narrow attending may be maladaptive and is sometimes associated with children with autism (Dube et al, 2016;Lovaas et al, 1971).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Functional communication was defined as the participant vocally stating one of the three preferred attention types (i.e., hand squeeze, massage, or high five) or escape from attention (i.e., “Go out, please”) while pointing to the communication card. Pointing to the communication card was part of the response requirement to ensure that Tyson attended to the communication board (i.e., as an observing response; Farber, Dickson, & Dube, 2017). During functional communication training, we collected data on the accuracy of responding.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%