1997
DOI: 10.1177/014272379701704905
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Referent introductions in Spanish narratives as a function of contextual constraints: a crosslinguistic perspective

Abstract: This study investigates referent introductions in narratives produced by Spanish children of 6, 9 and 11 years and adults in two situations, where they either could assume mutual background knowledge of the narrated content (Situation MK) or could not make this assumption (Situation NMK), while holding this content constant by means of a picture book. Overall, situation NMK elicits more indefinite first mentions than situation MK and this effect is more marked for the main characters than for the secondary one… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
20
1
2

Year Published

2002
2002
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
2
20
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…On the other hand, in Spanish casual speech, Bentivoglio and Weber (1986) and Bentivoglio (1993) show adult Spanish speakers' strong preference for introducing new referents in postverbal position. Furthermore, Kail and Sánchez y López (1997) have also found, at all ages, a strong preference to introduce characters in postverbal position in their study of narratives from Spanish monolingual subjects (ages 6, 9, 11 and adults).…”
Section: First Mention Of Animate Charactersmentioning
confidence: 86%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…On the other hand, in Spanish casual speech, Bentivoglio and Weber (1986) and Bentivoglio (1993) show adult Spanish speakers' strong preference for introducing new referents in postverbal position. Furthermore, Kail and Sánchez y López (1997) have also found, at all ages, a strong preference to introduce characters in postverbal position in their study of narratives from Spanish monolingual subjects (ages 6, 9, 11 and adults).…”
Section: First Mention Of Animate Charactersmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…(Mayer, 1969) was chosen as elicitation material due to the high number of different characters that need to be introduced in the story. This story has repeatedly been used in studies of narrative development in monolingual children (Bamberg, 1987;Kail and Hickmann, 1992;Berman and Slobin, 1994;Kail and Sánchez y López, 1997). The book contains 24 pictures in which a boy and his dog have a pet frog, which escapes one night from the jar where it was kept.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We will refer to this form of common ground as established knowledgeableness (Lee : 41). A number of studies (Menig‐Peterson ; Hudson and Shapiro ; Kail and Hickmann ; Kail and Sanchez y Lopez ) have shown that even 6 year olds take this established knowledgeableness into account when telling stories. In addition to this personal common ground, storytellers may use communal common ground to tailor their stories.…”
Section: Literature and Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Sonnenschein (1986:552) found that school-age children will provide more redundant information, in other words, additional explanations, to unfamiliar listeners to support listener understanding. Several studies indicated that referencing, in particular, will be influenced by shared knowledge with the listener (e.g., Kail and Hickmann 1992;Kail and Lopez 1997). It was found that when children and listeners could both see the picture, children were less likely to introduce referents.…”
Section: Shared Versus Unshared Information: Existing Evidencementioning
confidence: 99%