2018
DOI: 10.1525/collabra.119
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Relating Attentional Biases for Stimuli Associated with Social Reward and Punishment to Autistic Traits

Abstract: Evidence for impaired attention to social stimuli in autism has been mixed. The role of social feedback in shaping attention to other, non-social stimuli that are predictive of such feedback has not been examined in the context of autism. In the present study, participants searched for a color-defined target during a training phase, with the color of the target predicting the emotional reaction of a face that appeared after each trial. Then, participants performed visual search for a shape-defined target while… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
12
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

4
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
3
12
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Interestingly, and consistent with the principle of parsimony in the coupling of involuntary attentional control to biological needs, learning from reward and punishment appear to influence the control of attention via a common underlying mechanism. Behaviorally, reward learning and punishment learning typically have comparable effects on the orienting of attention (Anderson & Kim, 2018c; Watson et al, 2019). Compellingly, the neural correlates of attentional capture by previously reward-associated (see Anderson, 2019) and previously punishment-associated stimuli appear indistinguishable (Kim et al, in press).…”
Section: A Revised Model Of Adaptive Attentional Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Interestingly, and consistent with the principle of parsimony in the coupling of involuntary attentional control to biological needs, learning from reward and punishment appear to influence the control of attention via a common underlying mechanism. Behaviorally, reward learning and punishment learning typically have comparable effects on the orienting of attention (Anderson & Kim, 2018c; Watson et al, 2019). Compellingly, the neural correlates of attentional capture by previously reward-associated (see Anderson, 2019) and previously punishment-associated stimuli appear indistinguishable (Kim et al, in press).…”
Section: A Revised Model Of Adaptive Attentional Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Distraction by punishment-associated cues was also reported after conditioning with white noise (e.g., Koster et al, 2004; S. D. Smith et al, 2006), monetary loss (e.g., Wentura et al, 2014), or negative social feedback (Anderson, 2017; Anderson & Kim, 2018). Furthermore, oculomotor capture by punishment-related stimuli was demonstrated in eye-tracking studies (Mulckhuyse & Dalmaijer, 2016; Schmidt et al, 2015b).…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Consistent with this hypothesis, a variety of experiments have demonstrated attentional biases toward aversively conditioned stimuli. For example, stimuli previously associated with aversive electric shock (e.g., Schmidt, Belopolsky, & Theeuwes, 2015a;Wang, Yu, & Zhou, 2013), white noise (e.g., Koster, Crombez, Van Damme, Verschuere, & De Houwer, 2004;Smith, Most, Newsome, & Zald, 2006), monetary loss (e.g., Wentura, Müller, & Rothermund, 2014), or negative social feedback (Anderson, 2017;Anderson & Kim, 2018) during a conditioning phase impair performance on visual tasks, consistent with distraction by aversively conditioned stimuli. Furthermore, goal-directed eye movements are biased toward aversively conditioned stimuli, which are more frequently fixated when presented as task-irrelevant distractors compared with otherwise equivalent distractors without such association (Mulckhuyse, Crombez, & Van der Stigchel, 2013;Mulckhuyse & Dalmaijer, 2016;Schmidt, Belopolsky, & Theeuwes, 2015b).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%