980In the English language, various syntactic devices mark grammatical information in sentences. Such devices vary in how early they are mastered, and how susceptible they are to error in various populations. Three specific devices, in increasing order of both age of acquisition and susceptibility to error, are subject-verb-object (SVO) word order (A dog chases two balls), the regular plural morpheme (two balls), and the third person singular subject-verb agreement morpheme (A dog chases). This article examines whether these differences in acquisition order and susceptibility to error can be explained by a processing account involving working memory capacity and phonological ability.
Acquisition and Robustness of Syntactic Devices in EnglishWord order is very important in the English language. The canonical English SVO word order is mastered early in sentence comprehension, and it is used earlier and more strongly than regular plural and subject-verb agreement morphology MacWhinney, Bates, & Kliegl, 1984;McDonald, 2008). SVO word order has also proven to be a robustly maintained construction in diverse populations, such as aphasics (Wulfeck, Bates, & Capasso, 1991), late second-language learners (Johnson & Newport, 1989;McDonald, 2006), and adult native speakers under processing stress (Kilborn, 1991;McDonald, 2006).In English, the morphological markers of regular plural and third-person subject-verb agreement have identical nonstressed, nonsalient phonological realizations. Thus, phonological ability may play a larger role in their mastery than it does in the mastery of word order. Despite their identical phonological realizations, however, plural and agreement markers still differ in ease of acquisition and susceptibility to error in atypical populations. The regular plural morpheme is acquired before that of third-person agreement in production of native speakers (Brown, 1973) and in grammaticality judgment tasks (McDonald, 2008;Wulfeck, 1993). Earlier or better mastery of plural over agreement morphology has been seen in diverse populations, such as adults with aphasia ( Factors such as frequency of occurrence, semantic complexity (plural marks number; verb agreement marks number, person, and tense), and syntactic category (markings on verbs are considered more difficult than those on nouns) have been suggested as possible reasons for these differences (Brown, 1973;Goldschneider & DeKeyser, 2005;Wexler, 1994). In terms of a processing account, such factors may produce larger working memory demands for agreement constructions than for plural constructions. For example, in using verb agreement, the listener must maintain three types of information (number, agreement, tense) across the subject-verb phrase boundary, whereas the listener must maintain only number information within the noun phrase for plurals.If there is indeed an increased working memory demand for agreement constructions over plurals, processing such Differences in the cognitive demands of word order, plural, and subject-verb agreement construc...