2008
DOI: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.07.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Relationship between sleep/wake patterns, temperament and overall development in term infants over the first year of life

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

4
78
2
2

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 99 publications
(86 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
4
78
2
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Minimal relationships have been found previously between sleep and development, as measured by the Bayley (Scher, 2005;Spruyt et al, 2008). One previous study (Gibson et al, 2012) did find associations between sleep, as measured by actigraphy, for night sleep efficiency with fine motor skills and problem solving as measured by the Ages & Stages Questionnaire in a sample of one-year-olds.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 85%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Minimal relationships have been found previously between sleep and development, as measured by the Bayley (Scher, 2005;Spruyt et al, 2008). One previous study (Gibson et al, 2012) did find associations between sleep, as measured by actigraphy, for night sleep efficiency with fine motor skills and problem solving as measured by the Ages & Stages Questionnaire in a sample of one-year-olds.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…Overall, fragmented sleep was moderately associated (r = −.37) with lower mental development index scores, but no relationship was found with psychomotor development. A similar study of 20 infants (12 months) found no significant relationships between sleep measured via actigraphy and mental or motor developmental outcomes on the Bayley II (Spruyt et al, 2008). Gibson and colleagues also investigated the relationship between sleep and development in 51 one-year-olds (Gibson, Elder, & Gander, 2012).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, initially short sleepers had substantially less sleep at 0 to 1 years of age than typical sleepers (12:42 hours versus 14:34 hours) but comparable sleep from age 4 to 5 years, suggesting they gradually caught up with age. Compared with typical sleepers, these children had high rates of sleep problems (78.9%) and irritability, which often co-occur, 22 but they were otherwise similar in terms of sociodemographic profiles. Early sleep problems can reflect factors including maternal depression, parenting styles, child temperament (eg, irritability), and child feeding patterns and can promote shorter sleep.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Three studies are non-experimental. Spruyt et al ,23 in a detailed 12-month longitudinal study, report that children with easier temperament napped more readily, but at 12 months, decreased daytime sleep was associated with better emotional regulation as measured by the Bayley Scales of Infant Development. Two studies report association with internalising behavioural problems using the parent-form of the Child Behaviour Check List 17 25.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The difference between the experimental study and non-experimental studies are most likely explained by two processes. First, although the experimental study focuses on disruption of habitual napping,24 the non-experimental studies focus on normally occurring nap behaviours 17 23 25. Second, two of the non-experimental studies were of children who attended preschool settings 17 25.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%