2015
DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2015.1021256
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Remembering and telling self-consistent and self-discrepant memories

Abstract: It has been argued that memories that are inconsistent with one's self would differ from those that are consistent with the self. The present study addresses retrieval, phenomenology, rehearsal and narrative characteristics of autobiographical memories that are consistent versus discrepant with one's self. One hundred participants were asked to recall one self-consistent and one self-discrepant memory as well as an episode of telling these memories to others. They also filled out the Autobiographical Memory Qu… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

0
1
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(1 citation statement)
references
References 55 publications
0
1
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The CES was first introduced to measure the centrality of a traumatic or stressful event in a person’s identity and life story and how this related to PTSD symptoms (Berntsen & Rubin, 2006a). However, since its introduction, the CES has been used to examine the centrality of a wide range of events, including memories for events not directly associated with trauma or PTSD, such as memories of shameful events (e.g., Matos & Pinto-Gouveia, 2010, 2014), happy memories (e.g., Janssen, Hearne, & Takarangi, 2015; Zaragoza Scherman, Salgado, Shao, & Berntsen, 2015), self-discrepant memories (Mutlutürk & Tekcan, 2016) and memories of public events (Koppel, Brown, Stone, Coman, & Hirst, 2013). Likewise, the scale has been employed in a wide range of study populations, such as veterans (e.g., Brown, Antonius, Kramer, Root, & Hirst, 2010; Staugaard, Johannessen, Thomsen, Bertelsen, & Berntsen, 2015), older adults (e.g., Boals, Hayslip, Knowles, & Banks, 2012; O’Connor, Piet, & Hougaard, 2014), patients with schizophrenia (Allé et al, 2016; Berna et al, 2017) and patients with multiple sclerosis (Voltzenlogel et al, 2016).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The CES was first introduced to measure the centrality of a traumatic or stressful event in a person’s identity and life story and how this related to PTSD symptoms (Berntsen & Rubin, 2006a). However, since its introduction, the CES has been used to examine the centrality of a wide range of events, including memories for events not directly associated with trauma or PTSD, such as memories of shameful events (e.g., Matos & Pinto-Gouveia, 2010, 2014), happy memories (e.g., Janssen, Hearne, & Takarangi, 2015; Zaragoza Scherman, Salgado, Shao, & Berntsen, 2015), self-discrepant memories (Mutlutürk & Tekcan, 2016) and memories of public events (Koppel, Brown, Stone, Coman, & Hirst, 2013). Likewise, the scale has been employed in a wide range of study populations, such as veterans (e.g., Brown, Antonius, Kramer, Root, & Hirst, 2010; Staugaard, Johannessen, Thomsen, Bertelsen, & Berntsen, 2015), older adults (e.g., Boals, Hayslip, Knowles, & Banks, 2012; O’Connor, Piet, & Hougaard, 2014), patients with schizophrenia (Allé et al, 2016; Berna et al, 2017) and patients with multiple sclerosis (Voltzenlogel et al, 2016).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%