2012
DOI: 10.1007/s00265-012-1378-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reproductive allocation in female house wrens is not influenced by experimentally altered male attractiveness

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
12
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 71 publications
1
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, the increased body mass and excess production of sons on high-quality territories may be a product of (i) the current breeding territory having a direct effect on food availability for offspring, or (ii) differential allocation by females in relation to male attractiveness. We suspect that the latter scenario is unlikely, as recent experiments in our study population that have manipulated male attractiveness have found no effect on pre- and post-natal allocation by females (DeMory et al 2010; Grana et al 2012), suggesting that females do not differentially allocate offspring on the basis of male attractiveness. Nonetheless, separating territory quality from male attractiveness and intrasexual competitive ability experimentally could shed light on the factors contributing to the effect of territory quality on offspring condition and sex ratios.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…Thus, the increased body mass and excess production of sons on high-quality territories may be a product of (i) the current breeding territory having a direct effect on food availability for offspring, or (ii) differential allocation by females in relation to male attractiveness. We suspect that the latter scenario is unlikely, as recent experiments in our study population that have manipulated male attractiveness have found no effect on pre- and post-natal allocation by females (DeMory et al 2010; Grana et al 2012), suggesting that females do not differentially allocate offspring on the basis of male attractiveness. Nonetheless, separating territory quality from male attractiveness and intrasexual competitive ability experimentally could shed light on the factors contributing to the effect of territory quality on offspring condition and sex ratios.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…Individual growth rate r is assumed to be constant until the offspring reaches s c (table 1). The relationship between The critical size defining the end of the juvenile stage; after this size, offspring fitness is independent of size at birth s. This metric could also be expressed as a critical age (see Grana et al [29] for details). Varied from 5 to 20. r m…”
Section: Model Description and Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They are small (10–12 g), insectivorous, and prefer open woodland areas with dense herbaceous ground cover and few understory trees (Belles-Isles and Picman 1986; Finch 1989; Eckerle and Thompson 2006). Females typically select a mate based partly on the number and quality of potential nest-sites on his territory (Eckerle and Thompson 2006; Grana et al 2012). In the first brood (May–June), clutch size is 6–8 eggs (mode = 7 eggs), and in the second (July–August), 4–7 eggs (mode = 6 eggs).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Male settlement date was the date on which the male was heard singing near the nestbox, and at least 45–50 % of the bottom of the nestbox was covered with sticks (Eckerle and Thompson 2006; Grana et al 2012). Female settlement date was the date on which the female laid the first egg of her clutch (egg-1 day), and male time to pairing was defined as the difference between the female and male settlement dates (Eckerle and Thompson 2006; Grana et al 2012). …”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%