1981
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1981.tb03151.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Responses of Six-Month-Olds to the Distress of Their Peers

Abstract: Observations of 12 pairs of 6-month-old infants interacting in a laboratory playroom in the presence of both mothers indicated that the estimated frequency, duration, and temporal distribution of one infant's distressed vocalizations were statistically independent of those of the peer. The absence of toys in the environment was a reliable predictor of the extent of an infant's distress, whereas the peer's estimated frequency and duration of distress and the infant's own gender and locomotor status were not. In… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
29
1
1

Year Published

2011
2011
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
7
29
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Our results support only partially these hypotheses. Observing others' negative emotions induces autonomic arousal in both younger and older infants as has been previously shown [9], [12], [16], [18]. Peer's positive emotions triggered as well larger pupil diameter compared to the emotionally neutral states, although during limited time windows.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 55%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Our results support only partially these hypotheses. Observing others' negative emotions induces autonomic arousal in both younger and older infants as has been previously shown [9], [12], [16], [18]. Peer's positive emotions triggered as well larger pupil diameter compared to the emotionally neutral states, although during limited time windows.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 55%
“…In all three videos, the infants were recorded from the upper body part/torso and head, in a supine position. Infants' vocal and facial displays are socially relevant stimuli and have been previously shown to trigger emotional responses in their peers [18], [46].…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…While newborns display uninhibited emotional contagion (they cry when hearing other babies cry), already at the age of six months they become capable of distinguishing the distress of others from their own emotional state (Hay et al 1981). They then move to more complex manifestations of both emotional and cognitive empathy (Roth-Hanania et al., 2011), with the age of two years serving as a key developmental milestone for empathy and prosocial development.…”
Section: Ontogeny Of Empathymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet, this kind of developmental continuity cannot be taken for granted, especially if the nature of the abilities present in infants is qualitatively different from that present later (for instance “friend-and-foe-detection” vs. impartial concepts of right and wrong). The neonatal empathic cry – often taken as a prime example of an innate moral propensity (although see Campos et al [2008]) – appears to disappear over the course of the first year, before prosocial, behavioral responses to distress emerge in the second year of life (Hay, Nash, & Pedersen, 1981; Hoffman, 2007; Sagi & Hoffman, 1976). To my knowledge, only one study has investigated and documented a relation between infant performance on a socio-cognitive looking time task and socio-cognitive abilities (performance on a theory of mind task) in preschoolers (Yamaguchi, Kuhlmeier, Wynn, & vanMarle, 2009).…”
Section: Developmental Predecessors and Successorsmentioning
confidence: 99%