1997
DOI: 10.2307/1131846
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Restricting a Familiar Name in Response to Learning a New One: Evidence for the Mutual Exclusivity Bias in Young Two-Year-Olds

Abstract: Children under 2 1/2 years old tend to interpret novel words in accordance with the Mutual Exclusivity Principle, but tend not to reinterpret familiar words this way. Because alternative principles have been proposed that only predict the novel word effects, and because tests of the familiar word effects may have been flawed, a new test was administered. In Experiment 1 (N = 32), 24- to 25-month-olds heard stories in which a novel noun was used for an atypical exemplar of a familiar noun. When asked to select … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

1998
1998
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…And how is this knowledge brought to bear and realized in real time when a child encounters a real speaker and a novel object? Increasing evidence suggests that the answers may be found in the operating characteristics of perceiving and remembering (Merriman & Stevenson, 1997;Roberts & Jacob, 1991;Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996;Smith, Jones, & Landau, 1996;Stager & Werker, 1997). We brießy consider three lines of evidence from our own work.…”
Section: Early Word Learningmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…And how is this knowledge brought to bear and realized in real time when a child encounters a real speaker and a novel object? Increasing evidence suggests that the answers may be found in the operating characteristics of perceiving and remembering (Merriman & Stevenson, 1997;Roberts & Jacob, 1991;Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996;Smith, Jones, & Landau, 1996;Stager & Werker, 1997). We brießy consider three lines of evidence from our own work.…”
Section: Early Word Learningmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…However, there is reason to believe that such principles are themselves emergent properties of the cognitive system. For example, Merriman and Stevenson (1997) have argued that the tendency to avoid learning two names for the same object emerges naturally from the competition (MacWhinney, 1989) between closely-related lexical items.…”
Section: The Emergence Of the First Wordsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Only two types of previous studies have addressed the mechanisms by which young children identify and learn from such exemplars: investigations of whether hearing a new label for an object reduced children's tendency to apply a familiar label to it (Diesendruck et al, 1998;Gelman et al, 1989;Merriman, 1986;Merriman & Stevenson, 1997);and Merriman et al's (1995a) demonstrations that passing over an object during name training reduced 3-year-olds' tendency to select it in a forcedchoice but not a free-choice test of name generalization. The current investigation replicated and extended the latter set of findings in several important respects.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We propose that youngsters tend to identify an object as a negative exemplar of a familiar noun if the object is encountered in a situation in which a speaker is expected to label it, but does not. This process may be part of the reason for the decline that has been observed in youngsters' willingness to accept a familiar name for something after they have heard a different name used for it (Diesendruck, Gelman, & Lebowitz, 1998;Gelman, Wilcox, & Clark, 1989;Merriman, 1986;Merriman & Stevenson, 1997; but see Taylor & Gelman, 1989). However, given children's default assumption that labels have mutually exclusive extensions (Markman, 1989;Merriman & Bowman, 1989), this decline may derive as much from occurrence of the unexpected label as from failure of the expected label to occur.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%