1980
DOI: 10.3758/bf03199628
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Reward sequence and reinforcement level as determinants of S− behavior in differential conditioning

Abstract: In two differential conditioning experiments, groups of 10 rats each differed with respect to average reward and schedule of reward received in S+. Nonreward (N) occurred on all Strials. In both experiments, extinction of responding to S-(resistance to discrimination) was extensively regulated by reward sequence and was largely independent of average reward. In Experiment I, resistance to discrimination was a function of transitions from N to rewarded (R) trials (N-R transitions). In Experiment 2, resistance t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3

Citation Types

2
11
0

Year Published

1982
1982
1991
1991

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
2
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This conclusion, suggested by the present data, is consistent with previous findings. First, in conventional discrimination-learning situations, in which external cues such as brightness cues are relevant and hedonic memories are irrelevant, rats nevertheless employ hedonic memories (e.g., Capaldi, Berg, & Morris, 1975;Haggbloom, 1980;Haggbloom & Tillman, 1980). And in such conventional discrimination-learning situations, hedonic memory can have the same effect as in the unconventional discrimination-learning situation employed here: it can greatly elevate speed of responding on S-trials.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 86%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This conclusion, suggested by the present data, is consistent with previous findings. First, in conventional discrimination-learning situations, in which external cues such as brightness cues are relevant and hedonic memories are irrelevant, rats nevertheless employ hedonic memories (e.g., Capaldi, Berg, & Morris, 1975;Haggbloom, 1980;Haggbloom & Tillman, 1980). And in such conventional discrimination-learning situations, hedonic memory can have the same effect as in the unconventional discrimination-learning situation employed here: it can greatly elevate speed of responding on S-trials.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…Retrieving the memory of 20 pellets on Trial 3 of the 20-0-20 series apparently resulted in that memory's elevating speed on Trial 2 of that series. 351 Rats remember hedonic events such as reinforcement and nonreinforcement, and these memories can exercise considerable control over responding in a wide variety of instrumental learning tasks (e.g., Capaldi, 1966;Capaldi, Verry, & Davidson, 1980;Grosslight & Radlow, 19S6;Haggbloom, 1980;Haggbloom & Tillman, 1980;Seybert, Mellgren, Jobe, & Eckert, 1974).Understanding this behavioral control, however, depends upon how many hedonic events are remembered in particular situations: the animal may remember only the immediately prior hedonic event (single-event hedonic memory) or two or more prior hedonic events (multiple-event hedonic memory). The single-event hypothesis has proved capable of explaining a widevariety of instrumental learning effects (see, e.g., Capaldi, Nawrocki, & Verry, 1982;Capaldi et a1., 1980).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This follows from a consideration of the effects of training level on N-R transitions that occur from a consistently nonrewarded alley of one brightness, for example, black, to a consistently rewarded alley of another brightness, for example, white. Early in training, such transitions will result in O-pellet cues' acquiring strong control over responding; they will increase resistance to extinction in the S+ alternative or decrease vigor of responding in the S-alternative (see, e.g., Capaldi, Berg, & Morris, 1975;Capaldi et al, 1984;Haggbloom, 1980b). Such evidence clearly suggests that brightness cues are not intense enough to seriously overshadow 0-pellet cues.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The shift to an extinction series employed here, however, has a marked advantage: N-R transitions have been examined in a wide variety of more orthodox instrumental learning situations and with effects similar to those obtained here. Thus, N-R transitions have been shown to reduce discriminative responding (e.g., Capaldi et al, 1975;Capaldi et al, 1984;Haggbloom, 1980bHaggbloom, , 1982, to retard reversal learning in discrimination tasks (e.g., Grosslight & Radlow, 1956;Haggbloom & Tillman, 1980), to reduce the simultaneous and successive negative contrast effects (Campbell & Meyer, 1971;Capaldi & Ziff, 1969), and to elevate resistance to extinction: in punishment situations (Capaldi & Levy, 1972), in escape situations (Seybert, lobe, & Eckert, 1974), in the S+ alternative of discrimination tasks (e.g., Capaldi et aI., 1975;Haggbloom, 1980b), and in reward schedule situations in animals (e.g., Capaldi, 1964Capaldi, , 1967Leonard, 1969) and people (e.g., Grosslight, Hall, & Murin, 1953). Similarities of the sort noted above support the view that serial learning, whatever its unique characteristics, may be viewed as continuous with various more orthodox instrumental learning situations.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation