claims that the origin of early contrastive rhetoric lies in the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis of linguistic relativity. This article argues that her claim does not appear tenable. The Sapir-Whorf view of language as a causal determination is not compatible with Kaplan's (1966) position that rhetoric is evolved out of a culture. Furthermore, the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis is actually rooted in German ideas which also conflict with Kaplan's view. On the other hand, Hymetr' ethnography of communication approach can be seen as an important historical antecedent for contrastive rhetoric. Kaplan's view appears to reinforce Hymes' position that it is culture rather than the linguistic form which is the frame of reference for the use of language.Since Kaplan (1966) published his seminal article on "Cultural thought patterns in inter-cultural education", contrastive rhetoric has seen significant growth in second language writing research. Recent publications on contrastive rhetoric include books (e.g. . Although a great deal of attention has been paid to the rhetorical patterns in cross-cultural settings in contrastive rhetoric research (see Connor 19% €or a review), the issue of the origin of contrastive rhetoric has not attracted much attention until recently.In her recent article, Connor (1997: 199) claims that the basis for the principles of early contrastive rhetoric lies in the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis of linguistic relativity. In her book on contrastive rhetoric, Connor (1996: 10) expresses the same view: "The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis of linguistic relativity is basic to contrastive rhetoric because it suggests that different languages affect perception and thought in different ways". Connor's claim has not gone unnoticed. Leki (1997: 235) pointed out that interaction. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. tual analysis of a Chinese Hui narrative. Semwtica 115: 345-60. Ying, H. (1997) The equilibrium of yin and yang and dialogics of silence: a tex-[