2003
DOI: 10.3354/meps261217
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Role of olfactory appendages in chemically mediated orientation of blue crabs

Abstract: Benthic crustaceans such as the blue crab Callinectes sapidus use various sensory appendages to navigate chemical plumes. We characterized the role of different sensory structures in blue crabs during olfactory search by deafferenting (i.e. removing or rendering inactive) particular sensor populations and by quantifying odor-plume structure and flow dynamics. Our results indicate that blue crabs use both cephalic and thoracic appendages for olfactory-mediated orientation. Cephalic chemosensor deafferentation d… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
66
2
1

Year Published

2006
2006
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 82 publications
(74 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
5
66
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This conclusion is supported by a paired t test assuming equal variances (net-to-gross-displacement-ratio (path linearity) equaled 0.856 ± 0.144 (mean ± SD) and 0.814 ± 0.101 for untreated and blindfolded crabs, respectively; p = 0.44, N = 20, 13, respectively; walking speed equaled 7.394 ± 2.010 cm s -1 and 5.713 ± 2.612 cm s -1 for untreated and blindfolded crabs, respectively; p = 0.14, N = 20, 13, respectively). These values are comparable with those observed over many experiments with blue crabs in similar behavioral trials (Weissburg and Dusenbery 2002;Keller et al 2003;Jackson et al 2007). Percent success of crabs with blindfolds was again similar to crabs without blindfolds with untreated crabs achieving 40% success (see Jackson et al 2007) and blindfolded crabs achieving 46% success of finding the stimulus (N = 6 out of 13).…”
Section: Materials and Proceduressupporting
confidence: 77%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…This conclusion is supported by a paired t test assuming equal variances (net-to-gross-displacement-ratio (path linearity) equaled 0.856 ± 0.144 (mean ± SD) and 0.814 ± 0.101 for untreated and blindfolded crabs, respectively; p = 0.44, N = 20, 13, respectively; walking speed equaled 7.394 ± 2.010 cm s -1 and 5.713 ± 2.612 cm s -1 for untreated and blindfolded crabs, respectively; p = 0.14, N = 20, 13, respectively). These values are comparable with those observed over many experiments with blue crabs in similar behavioral trials (Weissburg and Dusenbery 2002;Keller et al 2003;Jackson et al 2007). Percent success of crabs with blindfolds was again similar to crabs without blindfolds with untreated crabs achieving 40% success (see Jackson et al 2007) and blindfolded crabs achieving 46% success of finding the stimulus (N = 6 out of 13).…”
Section: Materials and Proceduressupporting
confidence: 77%
“…The attractant had neutral buoyancy and was created by soaking 2.21 g of previously frozen shrimp per liter of seawater (taken from the flume sump) for 1 h. This general preparation has been used in many behavioral assays involving aquatic organisms and, in particular, blue crabs have effectively tracked sources of chemical exudates of shrimp in previous behavioral trials (Keller et al 2003;Jackson et al 2007). The specific concentration of stimulus was chosen as a value in the middle of the dose-response range of blue crabs.…”
Section: Materials and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This general preparation has been used in many behavioral assays involving aquatic organisms and, in particular, blue crabs have effectively tracked sources of chemical exudates of shrimp in previous behavior trails (Jackson et al, 2007;Keller et al, 2003). The stimulus solution was mixed with a fluorescent dye, Rhodamine 6G, which served as an optically measurable proxy for the relative concentration of the attractive odorant.…”
Section: Chemical Signal Preparationmentioning
confidence: 99%