2011
DOI: 10.1016/s1755-5345(13)70017-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Scenario adjustment in stated preference research

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
12
0
1

Year Published

2011
2011
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
0
12
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Baseline conditions and changes, as well as other components of the valuation scenario, may include subjective perceptions by respondents (e.g., perceived versus actual health risks) that require elicitation and communication to avoid scenario rejection or adjustment (Adamowicz et al 1997(Adamowicz et al , 2014Cameron, DeShazo, and Johnson 2011). Scenario design must take subjective perceptions into consideration when describing the actual change to be valued.…”
Section: Scenario Descriptionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Baseline conditions and changes, as well as other components of the valuation scenario, may include subjective perceptions by respondents (e.g., perceived versus actual health risks) that require elicitation and communication to avoid scenario rejection or adjustment (Adamowicz et al 1997(Adamowicz et al , 2014Cameron, DeShazo, and Johnson 2011). Scenario design must take subjective perceptions into consideration when describing the actual change to be valued.…”
Section: Scenario Descriptionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…SP studies, this is often conceptualized as a case of risk, in which a probability distribution of possible states-of-the-world is known or can be approximated. A growing literature illustrates the relevance of risk and uncertainty (e.g., over the change in an environmental outcome) for SP studies (e.g., Roberts, Boyer, and Lusk 2008;Hanley, Kriström, and Shogren 2009;Shaw and Baker 2010;Cameron et al 2011;Glenk andColombo 2011, 2013;Akter, Bennett, and Ward 2012;Rolfe and Windle 2015), following similar relevance for welfare analysis in general (Graham 1981). This work demonstrates that the omission of relevant risk information from scenarios (i.e., treating risky outcomes as certain) can lead to empirical value estimates that do not reflect ex ante welfare change under uncertainty.…”
Section: Scenario Descriptionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…As a case in point, stated preference data can be vulnerable to bias because (a) it often provides respondents with more information about hypothetical scenarios than they would have access to in the real world (TRB, 2000, p. 26), and (b) respondents may be skeptical about the realism of certain scenarios (Cameron, Deshazo, & Johnson, 2010). Several researchers have suggested that stated preference experiments can be made more reliable by using them in combination with other survey tools.…”
Section: Industry Examples and Framework For Actionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Additionally, as addressed in Cameron et al (2007), our survey was designed to elicit useful information about preferences over the stated health scenarios through tutorials and careful explanations about how the choice tasks should be approached. However, the potential for respondents to subjectively adjust the choice scenarios to more closely reflect their own situation was assessed through follow-up questions.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%