2019
DOI: 10.1002/pam.22135
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

School Starting Age and Cognitive Development

Abstract: We present evidence of a positive relationship between school starting age and children's cognitive development from ages 6 to 18 using a fuzzy regression discontinuity design and large‐scale population‐level birth and school data from the state of Florida. We estimate effects of being old for grade (being born in September vs. August) that are remarkably stable—always around 0.2 SD difference in test scores—across a wide range of heterogeneous groups, based on maternal education, poverty at birth, race/ethnic… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

4
44
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 67 publications
(48 citation statements)
references
References 87 publications
(140 reference statements)
4
44
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Again, the difference at the cutoff is entirely driven by diagnoses of behavioral problems, as we find no effect for learning impairments. Interestingly, the point estimates for behavioral problems are quite similar to those found for Florida by Dhuey et al (), who show that September‐born children have a 2.5–3.4% higher probability of being classified as having a “behavioral disability” (see Dhuey et al, , table 4, p. 555).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 82%
“…Again, the difference at the cutoff is entirely driven by diagnoses of behavioral problems, as we find no effect for learning impairments. Interestingly, the point estimates for behavioral problems are quite similar to those found for Florida by Dhuey et al (), who show that September‐born children have a 2.5–3.4% higher probability of being classified as having a “behavioral disability” (see Dhuey et al, , table 4, p. 555).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 82%
“…The schoolchildren who delay their enrolment in a school year score significantly higher than schoolchildren enrolled in their corresponding cohort [28]. In the direction of this research, it can be asserted that relatively older schoolchildren in a primary school class (6-12 years) score significantly higher on academic achievement tests than their relatively younger peers [13,29,30].…”
Section: Relative Age Effects On Academic Performancementioning
confidence: 77%
“…If the coexisting variable with relative age is a person's migration status in the country (demographic variable), immigrant or non-immigrant, it can be seen that relatively younger and immigrant schoolchildren obtain significantly lower mean scores than nonimmigrant schoolchildren of the same relative age [6]. Finally, an adequate family climate of support and involvement in the education of relatively younger schoolchildren causes the RAE on academic performance to be significantly smaller than those of schoolchildren of the same relative age but with an inadequate family climate [6,28].…”
Section: Relative Age Effects On Academic Performancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, one solution to provide a causal impact of school starting age, the endogeneity issues mentioned must be considered. Some studies have studied the impact of school starting age on different outcomes such as earnings, educational attainment, criminal involvement, cognitive development, and mental health (Angrist & Krueger, 1992;Cook & Kang, 2016;Dee & Sievertsen, 2018;Dhuey et al, 2019;Smith, 2009). Some researchers argue that schooling at a younger age is beneficial for improving academic performances (Pellizzari & Billari, 2012) securing higher scores on cognitive tests, and having fewer behavioral problems than older students (Mayer, 1999).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%