2001
DOI: 10.1038/35053167
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Self-recognition and the right hemisphere

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

18
156
1
5

Year Published

2005
2005
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 304 publications
(180 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
18
156
1
5
Order By: Relevance
“…The subjects' task was to indicate as quickly as possible which face it was by pressing one of three buttons with their right hand in one condition and left in another (resulting in six conditions-three types of faces times two hands). Results indicated a significant reaction time advantage for the self faces when subjects pressed the button with their left hand, which (2000); Keenan et al (1999Keenan et al ( , 2000Keenan et al ( , 2001Keenan et al ( , 2003; Kircher et al (2000Kircher et al ( , 2001; Turk et al Functional uniqueness/ independence Reed & Farah (1995) Self-reference effect (various authors); Klein et al (2002) De Renzi et al (1987); Hodges & McCarthy (1993) Species specificity Gallup (1970Gallup ( , 1979; Suarez & Gallup (1981) is primarily controlled by the right hemisphere-that is, their reaction times for this condition were significantly faster than their reaction times in any of the other five conditions. In another study, subjects were shown morphed images of their own face and the face of a famous person (Keenan et al, 2000), similar to the design used in Keenan et al's (2003) patient study.…”
Section: Face Recognitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The subjects' task was to indicate as quickly as possible which face it was by pressing one of three buttons with their right hand in one condition and left in another (resulting in six conditions-three types of faces times two hands). Results indicated a significant reaction time advantage for the self faces when subjects pressed the button with their left hand, which (2000); Keenan et al (1999Keenan et al ( , 2000Keenan et al ( , 2001Keenan et al ( , 2003; Kircher et al (2000Kircher et al ( , 2001; Turk et al Functional uniqueness/ independence Reed & Farah (1995) Self-reference effect (various authors); Klein et al (2002) De Renzi et al (1987); Hodges & McCarthy (1993) Species specificity Gallup (1970Gallup ( , 1979; Suarez & Gallup (1981) is primarily controlled by the right hemisphere-that is, their reaction times for this condition were significantly faster than their reaction times in any of the other five conditions. In another study, subjects were shown morphed images of their own face and the face of a famous person (Keenan et al, 2000), similar to the design used in Keenan et al's (2003) patient study.…”
Section: Face Recognitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Supramarginal gyrus, superior parietal lobule, precuneus Keenan et al (1999) Hand difference in reaction time for self-face recognition Unspecified right hemisphere Keenan et al (2000) Hand difference in reaction time for self-face recognition Unspecified right hemisphere Keenan et al (2001) Hemispheric difference in self-face recognition Unspecified right hemisphere Platek & Gallup (2002) Hand difference in reaction time for self-face recognition Unspecified right hemisphere Turk et al (2002) Hemispheric difference in self-face recognition in split-brain patient Unspecified left hemisphere Kircher et al (2000Kircher et al ( , 2001 Greater activation for self vs. partner face recognition representations used to discriminate own and other bodies (be these representations separate or integral) are precise. Within clinical neurology, there are many disorders of bodily sensation, action, and awareness (see Goldenberg, 2003, for a review), the most relevant being autotopagnosia and asomatognosia.…”
Section: Anterior Cingulate Gyrus Fusiform Gyrusmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent years have seen a renewed interest of neurobiological research into the self and how it is grounded in mechanisms of bodily perception and cognition [Gallagher, 2000;Jeannerod, 2003;Vogeley and Fink, 2003]. The human body (of others as well as one's own) provides a particularly rich source of perceptual information and recent neuroimaging research has unraveled several brain mechanisms involved in the visual perception and recognition of human bodies [Allison et al, 2000;Downing et al, 2001;Thierry et al, 2006;Urgesi et al, 2007], either of one's own or of others [Ehrsson et al, 2004;Keenan et al, 2001;Sugiura et al, 2005]. These studies revealed the existence of an extended cortical network encompassing extrastriate areas, temporoparietal and parietal cortex, as well as medial and lateral frontal regions.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Early imaging studies focused on stereotypes (Hart et al, 2000;Phelps et al, 2000), self-knowledge (Kelley et al, 2002), and theory of mind (Baron-Cohen et al, 1994;Frith and Frith, 1999); however, work has now extended into several areas of social psychological inquiry including self-serving biases (Blackwood et al, 2003), self-awareness (Gusnard et al, 2001;Keenan et al, 2001;Eisenberger et al, in press), judgment and decision-making (De Quervain et al, 2004;Sanfey et al, 2003), cooperation (Kosfeld et al, 2005;Rilling et al, 2004), selfschemas , person knowledge (Mitchell et al, 2004a), social exclusion (Eisenberger et al, 2003), attitudinal evaluation (Cunningham et al, 2003;Wood et al, 2005), regulation of stereotypes (Amodio et al, 2003;Richeson et al, 2003;Wheeler and Fiske, 2005), expectancy effects Petrovic et al, 2002;Wager et al, 2004), relational cognition (Aron et al, 2005;Iacoboni et al, 2004), empathy (Carr et al, 2003;Singer et al, 2004), and emotional reappraisal (Beauregard et al, 2001;Ochsner et al, 2002). This special issue devoted to social cognitive neuroscience brings new light to these existing themes (Cunningham, Espinet, DeYoung, and Zelazo, this issue; Mitchell, Banaji, and Macrae, this issue; Ochsner et al, this issue; Sander et al, this issue) and tackles areas new to social cognitive neuroscience such as attribution (Harris, Todorov, and Fiske, this issue; Heberlein and Saxe, this issue), attachment (Gillath, Bunge, Shaver, Wendelken, and Mikulincer, this issue), self-esteem (Pruessner et al, this issue), and intention planning …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%