1990
DOI: 10.3758/bf03213882
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Semantic priming in the pronunciation task: The role of prospective prime-generated expectancies

Abstract: In the relatedness proportion effect, semantic priming increases with an increase in the probability that a word prime will be followed by a semantically related word target. This effect has frequently been obtained in the lexical decision task but not in a pronunciation task. In the present experiment, relatedness proportion was manipulated in two pronunciation tasks, one with and one without nonword targets, using category names as primes. In both tasks, a relatedness proportion effect occurred for high-domi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
111
1
1

Year Published

1993
1993
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 108 publications
(123 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
10
111
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In the relatedness proportion effect, the size of semantic priming effects increases as the proportion of related word pairs relative to unrelated word pairs in the stimulus list increases. This modulation has been reported both with a lexical decision task (e.g., De Groot, 1984;Seidenberg, Waters, Sanders, & Langer, 1984;Shelton & Martin, 1992) and a pronunciation task (Keefe & Neely, 1990). It occurs for long but not short prime-target stimulus-onset asynchronies (e.g., Den Heyer, Briand, & Dannebring, 1983;but see De Groot, 1984).…”
mentioning
confidence: 66%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In the relatedness proportion effect, the size of semantic priming effects increases as the proportion of related word pairs relative to unrelated word pairs in the stimulus list increases. This modulation has been reported both with a lexical decision task (e.g., De Groot, 1984;Seidenberg, Waters, Sanders, & Langer, 1984;Shelton & Martin, 1992) and a pronunciation task (Keefe & Neely, 1990). It occurs for long but not short prime-target stimulus-onset asynchronies (e.g., Den Heyer, Briand, & Dannebring, 1983;but see De Groot, 1984).…”
mentioning
confidence: 66%
“…It should be noted here that these task characteristics are probably not specific to lexical decision, since relatedness proportion RT effects have also been reported when using the pronunciation task (cf. Keefe & Neely, 1990). In other words, the presence of a binary decision component in the lexical decision task is insufficient explanation for the occurrence of expectancy-induced effects.…”
Section: Effects Specific To Reaction-time Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Although it is often argued that a naming task reflects automatic priming to a greater extent than lexical decision (Balota & Chumbley, 1985;Seidenberg et al, 1984), recent studies have suggested that it may be susceptible to some of the same strategic influences (Keefe & Neely, 1990). Instead, we decided to stick with the lexical-decision task, although we used a single-word presentation procedure, rather than the standard method, in which each prime and target is presented as a distinct pair.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As I have argued elsewhere (Hagoort, 1993), there is convincing evidence (Balota & Chumbley, 1984;De Groot, 1984;De Groot, Thomassen, & Hudson, 1986;Keefe & Neely, 1990;Neely, 1977Neely, , 1991Neely, Keefe, & Ross, 1989;Seidenberg, Waters, Sanders, & Langer, 1984) that priming effects can be attributed to both automatic and more controlled priming mechanisms.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%